首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   31898篇
  免费   1609篇
  国内免费   38篇
电工技术   338篇
综合类   59篇
化学工业   6887篇
金属工艺   600篇
机械仪表   579篇
建筑科学   1888篇
矿业工程   78篇
能源动力   873篇
轻工业   2697篇
水利工程   297篇
石油天然气   178篇
武器工业   4篇
无线电   2294篇
一般工业技术   5920篇
冶金工业   5268篇
原子能技术   207篇
自动化技术   5378篇
  2023年   259篇
  2022年   356篇
  2021年   699篇
  2020年   513篇
  2019年   594篇
  2018年   725篇
  2017年   694篇
  2016年   912篇
  2015年   820篇
  2014年   1031篇
  2013年   1933篇
  2012年   1677篇
  2011年   2145篇
  2010年   1523篇
  2009年   1474篇
  2008年   1774篇
  2007年   1651篇
  2006年   1447篇
  2005年   1235篇
  2004年   1097篇
  2003年   949篇
  2002年   916篇
  2001年   544篇
  2000年   524篇
  1999年   534篇
  1998年   493篇
  1997年   451篇
  1996年   462篇
  1995年   439篇
  1994年   428篇
  1993年   430篇
  1992年   384篇
  1991年   229篇
  1990年   319篇
  1989年   322篇
  1988年   272篇
  1987年   296篇
  1986年   286篇
  1985年   306篇
  1984年   267篇
  1983年   257篇
  1982年   239篇
  1981年   210篇
  1980年   175篇
  1979年   182篇
  1978年   165篇
  1977年   136篇
  1976年   122篇
  1975年   134篇
  1974年   109篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
This study examined organizational family-responsive policies, perceptions of the organization as family supportive, and supervisor support as issues that may be salient to the experience of conflict between paid employment (work) and family roles. Data were collected from 355 managerial personnel in New Zealand. Although work-family conflict and psychological strain were strongly linked, the availability of organizational policies had no significant association with levels of conflict or strain, whereas policy usage was related only to work-to-family interference and not to family-to-work interference. On the other hand, perceptions of the organization as family supportive and supervisor support for work-family balance displayed significant relationships with key variables, highlighting the importance of these variables for interventions designed to ameliorate the negative impact of work-family conflict on managerial well-being. Implications for the effective implementation of family-responsive interventions are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
132.
133.
Many regulators use benchmarking as part of the incentive-based regulation of natural monopolies so as to reduce the possibilities for gaming. However, the use of benchmarking can lead firms to pursue virtual rather than true performance improvements by gaming the regulator’s benchmarking in a number of ways that are contrary to the intentions of the scheme.  相似文献   
134.
135.
The time course of spoken word recognition depends largely on the frequencies of a word and its competitors, or neighbors (similar-sounding words). However, variability in natural lexicons makes systematic analysis of frequency and neighbor similarity difficult. Artificial lexicons were used to achieve precise control over word frequency and phonological similarity. Eye tracking provided time course measures of lexical activation and competition (during spoken instructions to perform visually guided tasks) both during and after word learning, as a function of word frequency, neighbor type, and neighbor frequency. Apparent shifts from holistic to incremental competitor effects were observed in adults and neural network simulations, suggesting such shifts reflect general properties of learning rather than changes in the nature of lexical representations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
136.
Fodor and Pylyshyn (1988) have argued that the cognitive architecture is not Connectionist. Their argument takes the following form: (1) the cognitive architecture is Classical; (2) Classicalism and Connectionism are incompatible; (3) therefore the cognitive architecture is not Connectionist. In this essay I argue that Fodor and Pylyshyn's defenses of (1) and (2) are inadequate. Their argument for (1), based on their claim that Classicalism best explains the systematicity of cognitive capacities, is an invalid instance of inference to the best explanation. And their argument for (2) turns out to be question-begging. The upshot is that, while Fodor and Pylyshyn have presented Connectionists with the important empirical challenge of explaining systematicity, they have failed to provide sufficient reason for inferring that the cognitive architecture is Classical and not Connectionist.  相似文献   
137.
Modulo scheduling theory can be applied successfully to overlap Fortran DO loops on pipelined computers issuing multiple operations per cycle both with and without special loop architectural support. This paper shows that a broader class of loops—REPEAT-UNTIL, WHILE, and loops with more than one exit, in which the trip count is not known beforehand—can also be overlapped efficiently on multiple-issue pipelined machines. The approach is described with respect to a specific machine model, but it can be extended to other models. Special features in the architecture, as well as compiler representations for accelerating these loop constructs, are discussed. Performance results are presented for a few select examples.An earlier version of this paper was presented at Supercomputing '90.  相似文献   
138.
139.
This paper is concerned with structural and algorithmic aspects of certain R-bases in polynomial rings R[Xij] over a commutative ring R with 1. These bases are related to standard tableaux. We shall examine the main tools in full detail: (symmetrized) bideterminants, Capelli operators, hyperdominance, and generalized Laplace's expansions. These tools are then applied to the representation theory of symmetric groups. In particular, we present an algorithm which efficiently computes for every skew module of a symmetric group an R-basis which is adapted to a Specht series. This result is a constructive, characteristic-free analogue of the celebrated Littlewood-Richardson rule. This paper will serve as the basis for a possible generalization of that rule to more general shapes.  相似文献   
140.
Eight subjects studied a set of complex visual images after administration of 0.4 mg scopolamine. Another 8 subjects performed the same task without drug administration. On a subsequent item recognition test, subjects rated, on a 5-point scale, their confidence that the studied pictures and an equal number of unstudied lures were actually presented. Results showed that scopolamine affected responses to studied items, but not unstudied lures, demonstrating an unambiguous effect of scopolamine on recognition memory. To describe the scopolamine-injected subjects' data, the authors constructed a new model of 2-process recognition that includes the A. P. Yonelinas (1994) model as a limiting case. The model analysis suggests that scopolamine affected both familiarity and recollection. In particular, scopolamine did not affect the frequency with which recollection took place, but rather, affected the amount of recollected information. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号