首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   6969篇
  免费   229篇
  国内免费   15篇
电工技术   78篇
综合类   6篇
化学工业   1312篇
金属工艺   85篇
机械仪表   173篇
建筑科学   261篇
矿业工程   48篇
能源动力   159篇
轻工业   560篇
水利工程   103篇
石油天然气   16篇
无线电   386篇
一般工业技术   1130篇
冶金工业   1680篇
原子能技术   49篇
自动化技术   1167篇
  2023年   46篇
  2022年   79篇
  2021年   115篇
  2020年   87篇
  2019年   100篇
  2018年   149篇
  2017年   126篇
  2016年   124篇
  2015年   132篇
  2014年   185篇
  2013年   426篇
  2012年   343篇
  2011年   418篇
  2010年   294篇
  2009年   265篇
  2008年   369篇
  2007年   363篇
  2006年   313篇
  2005年   290篇
  2004年   248篇
  2003年   213篇
  2002年   220篇
  2001年   138篇
  2000年   109篇
  1999年   139篇
  1998年   131篇
  1997年   132篇
  1996年   123篇
  1995年   107篇
  1994年   117篇
  1993年   94篇
  1992年   115篇
  1991年   57篇
  1990年   104篇
  1989年   83篇
  1988年   77篇
  1987年   91篇
  1986年   61篇
  1985年   72篇
  1984年   60篇
  1983年   67篇
  1982年   60篇
  1981年   48篇
  1980年   45篇
  1979年   47篇
  1978年   37篇
  1977年   45篇
  1976年   29篇
  1975年   33篇
  1973年   19篇
排序方式: 共有7213条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
931.
932.
Argues that since it was first proposed, the therapeutic "frame" has been widely adopted in a relatively unquestioned manner despite major modifications in clinical theory and practice. A reexamination of the frame from a contemporary perspective is presented. While traditional conceptions of the frame emphasize its function for the client, it is argued that this structure also serves a crucial purpose for the therapist. By minimizing the opportunity for self-indulgence (e.g., by fostering abstinence, anonymity, and neutrality), the frame enables the clinician to maintain a therapeutic stance and to create an environment that fosters comprehension of the client. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
933.
28 21–40 yr old middle-class mothers and their noncompliant children, ages 2.6–6.6 yrs, participated in 1 of 3 treatment (social reinforcement, time-out, and social reinforcement plus time-out) conditions or a no-treatment control. Results indicate that in addition to producing greater reductions in noncompliant behavior, time-out is rated as comparable in acceptability to social reinforcement. (6 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
934.
38 undergraduate low self-monitors and 42 undergraduate high self-monitors (as determined by a scale of self-monitoring of expressive behavior) witnessed in small groups a staged crime of either their own wristwatches or a laboratory calculator. Campus police detectives took individual witness statements as if a real crime had occurred, and using biased or unbiased instructions, police administered a suspect-present photospread. Results show that victim witnesses who had been given biased instructions gave the least accurate identifications and that high self-monitoring Ss were least likely to reject the lineup when instructions were biased. (25 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
935.
Seven experiments examined the time course of primed fragment-completion performance. A pilot experiment and Experiment 1 showed that rapid forgetting occurs immediately after study for a period of approximately 5 min. The rate of this immediate forgetting is independent of the length of the list. Experiment 2 showed that priming effects were still present after 16 months. Experiments 3 and 4 provided further evidence of forgetting over 1 week. Experiment 5 showed that retention performance after 20 min is unaffected by the interpolated study and recall of other lists of words. Experiment 6 showed that 10-min retention performance was substantially reduced as list length was increased from 10 to 100 words; but it showed no evidence of intralist proactive interference. The combined results of the seven experiments illustrate some similarities and differences between forgetting in primed fragment completion and in episodic memory tasks such as recall and recognition. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
936.
937.
Relationships among stressful life events, negative affect, and judged quality of intimate relationships were explored. Three studies and a mini-meta-analysis revealed that as negative life events increased, judgments of close relationships gradually became less favorable, jumped back toward positivity, and then, again, gradually became less favorable. The same methods of analysis revealed a relationship between negative life events and negative affect with no evidence of significant discontinuities. Moreover, the correlation between relationship satisfaction and negative affect (with negative events held constant), was maximized at the point of relationship-judgment discontinuity. Although the findings are complex, they are consistent with the authors' theoretical account and represent an initial attempt to conceptualize the effect of negative life events in light of the recent social judgment literature. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
938.
Evaluated the validity of a prevalent model of attitude structure that specifies 3 components: affect, behavior, and cognition. Five conditions needed for properly testing the 3-component distinction were identified. Consideration of the tripartite model's theoretical basis indicated that the most important validating conditions are (a) the use of nonverbal, in addition to verbal, measures of affect and behavior; and (b) the physical presence of the attitude object. Study 1—in which 138 undergraduates attitudes toward snakes were examined, through the use of measures such as the Mood Adjective Check List, semantic differential, and distance of approach—indicated very strong support for this tripartite model. The model was statistically acceptable, its relative fit was very good, and the intercomponent correlations were moderate. Study 2, with 105 Ss, was a verbal report analog of Study 1. Results from Study 2 indicate that higher intercomponent correlations occurred when attitude measures derived solely from verbal reports and when the attitude object was not physically present. (74 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
939.
Previous work has shown that abrupt visual onsets capture attention. Possible mechanisms for this phenomenon include (1) luminance-change detection system and (2) a mechanism that detects the appearance of new perceptual objects. Exps 1 and 2 revealed that attention is captured in visual search by the appearance of a new perceptual object even when the object is equiluminant with its background and thus exhibits no luminance change when it appears. Exp 3 showed that a highly salient luminance increment alone is not sufficient to capture attention. These findings suggest that attentional capture is mediated by a mechanism that detects the appearance of new perceptual objects. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
940.
The relationships among bereavement, depressed mood, and immunologic patterns prognostic for the development of AIDS were determined in a sample of HIV seropositive gay men and a comparison group of HIV seronegative gay men. Immunologic parameters were assessed in 45 men who had recently experienced the deaths of close friends and 45 matched nonbereaved men. No immune differences were found between bereaved and nonbereaved men. Among the HIV seropositive nonbereaved men, higher depressed mood was significantly associated with fewer CD4 (helper/inducer) T lymphocytes, more activated CD8 (suppressor/cytotoxic) T cells, and lower proliferative responses to the mitogen phytohemagglutinin. In summary, HIV seropositive men who reported higher levels of depressed mood not associated with bereavement demonstrated immunologic patterns consistent with HIV activity and progression. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号