首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4178篇
  免费   0篇
化学工业   16篇
建筑科学   2篇
轻工业   2篇
无线电   4篇
一般工业技术   5篇
冶金工业   4136篇
原子能技术   1篇
自动化技术   12篇
  2017年   1篇
  2015年   2篇
  2014年   1篇
  2013年   7篇
  2012年   1篇
  2010年   1篇
  2008年   1篇
  2007年   1篇
  2006年   2篇
  2005年   1篇
  2004年   2篇
  2003年   6篇
  2002年   1篇
  2001年   1篇
  1999年   128篇
  1998年   1287篇
  1997年   712篇
  1996年   462篇
  1995年   266篇
  1994年   191篇
  1993年   260篇
  1992年   42篇
  1991年   49篇
  1990年   45篇
  1989年   71篇
  1988年   60篇
  1987年   64篇
  1986年   42篇
  1985年   49篇
  1983年   9篇
  1982年   17篇
  1981年   29篇
  1980年   27篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   6篇
  1977年   111篇
  1976年   211篇
  1975年   6篇
  1972年   1篇
  1964年   1篇
  1961年   1篇
  1955年   1篇
排序方式: 共有4178条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
992.
993.
994.
995.
We studied multiple determinants of graft survival at a single center and the effects of nonimmunologic graft loss on transplant survival. This retrospective study examined the results of 589 cadaver donor transplants performed between 1986 and 1992. Graft survival rates were calculated using Kaplan-Meier estimates for both overall graft survival (all causes of graft loss) and immunologic graft survival (function lost due to acute or chronic rejection and noncompliance). Cadaver graft survival was significantly poorer with an increasing degree of DR mismatch (P=0.02). An analysis of pretransplant variables showed graft loss risk was highest with greater DR mismatches, two B-antigen mismatch, higher donor serum creatinine, and younger recipient age. After transplantation, acute rejection was the most significant factor associated with long-term graft survival. Our data demonstrate a significant advantage for zero DR and one DR mismatch cadaver donor transplants, with excellent immunologic graft survival. This study suggests that a combination of immediate graft function, prevention of acute rejection by appropriate early immunosuppressive therapy, and acceptable DR match enhances cadaveric graft survival.  相似文献   
996.
Cross-adaptation, the decrease in sensitivity to one odorant following exposure to a different odorant, is affected by odorant similarity, both perceptual and structural, but the precise relationship is obscure. The present series of studies was designed to explore various aspects of perceptual and structural similarity as they relate to cross-adaptation. In Experiment 1, cross-adaptation was assessed between androstenone and five odorants that share a common urinous note with androstenone, but retain unique perceptual characteristics; only the compound judged most perceptually similar to androstenone cross-adapted it. In Experiment 2, odorants both perceptually and structurally similar (androstenone and androstanone) displayed significant, mutual cross-adaptation. Furthermore, magnitude estimates for androstanone were significantly reduced following exposure to 3-methylidene-5 alpha-androstane (3M5A), a structurally similar, perceptually odorless compound. This finding appears to be the first demonstration that an odorless compound can affect, via cross-adaptation, the perception of an odorous compound. Finally, in Experiment 3, significant, asymmetric cross-adaptation was observed between compounds that are perceptually and structurally dissimilar (4-cyclohexylcyclohexanone [4-CHCH] and androstenone). These findings indicate that the role of similarity in cross-adaptation is difficult to quantify and emphasize the numerous odorant characteristics that can affect cross-adaptation.  相似文献   
997.
998.
We report five cases presenting with soft tissue and bone overgrowth that demonstrate the ability of MRI to establish a diagnosis in the absence of specific clinical features. Disorders included macrodystrophia lipomatosa, angiolipomatosis, Klippel-Trenaunay-Weber syndrome, blue rubber bleb naevus syndrome and one case of segmental limited hypertrophy. The MRI appearances, and other radiological features of these conditions are discussed. MRI is recommended in all cases of macrodystrophy when the clinical features and plain film findings are indeterminate.  相似文献   
999.
Biotinylated lectins from Sambucus nigra (SNA) and Maackia amurensis (MAA), which bind to alpha 2,6-linked and alpha 2,3-linked sialyl residues, respectively, were used as probes to study glycan terminal modifications associated with decidualization in the uterine stroma of pregnant rats and mice. Binding of lectins from Erythrina cristagalli (ECA), Phaseolus vulgaris (leukoagglutinin, L-PHA), Triticum vulgaris (WGA) and Bandeiraea simplicifolia (BSA-1B4) was also examined. Tissues from rats between day 5 and day 8 of gestation and mice between day 5 and day 7 of gestation were fixed in Bouin's solution and embedded in wax prior to lectin histochemistry. On day 7 in rats and day 6 in mice, there was a marked reduction in the binding of SNA in the subluminal decidua surrounding the implantation site. In rats, MAA binding to enlarged decidual cells around the implantation chamber was increased markedly, but there was no change in mice. In both species there was de novo binding of ECA in the SNA-negative area, suggesting that the loss of alpha 2,6-linked sialyl residues unmasks terminal N-acetyl lactosamine. These findings are consistent with previous evidence of a close structural and functional similarity between the artificially induced deciduoma and true decidua of rats and show identical changes to the glycosylation patterns previously found in differentiating rat deciduoma. In both species, therefore, decidua exhibits regionally specific terminal glycosylation. However, the species-specific expression of alpha 2,3-linked sialyl residues suggests distinct patterns of steroidally modulated sialyl transferase expression.  相似文献   
1000.
The influences of bilateral or unilateral injuries within the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) upon spatial learning in a water maze were examined in three experiments. Place-learning and response-learning were investigated in a four-alley 'Greek-cross' shaped water maze with extra-maze visual cues available. No differences were detected on any of several measures sensitive to learning between the lesion groups on the place-learning task. Microanalysis of behavior within trials revealed that animals with either bilateral or right unilateral PPC injuries committed significantly more total errors, initial alley entrance ('reference memory') errors, and re-entry ('working memory') errors in the response-learning paradigm than did either the control or left PPC-injured rats. No differences were detected between the latter two groups on these measures. Unilateral lesions resulted in asymmetrical placing responses ipsilateral to the injury 10 days after surgery whereas bilateral injuries resulted in asymmetrical placing with mixed directionality. The acquisition of the response-learning problem in the absence of visual cues was studied on animals prepared with unilateral lesions and housed post-operatively either in isolation or in a 'complex environment.' In the absence of visual cues both right and left PPC-injured rats committed more errors than sham controls, and differential post-surgical housing did not attenuate these impairments. These same animals were trained on the landmark navigation task. Although no differences appeared between the lesion groups, a generalized but transient facilitation of learning was observed in animals housed in the 'complex' environment. Unilateral injuries placed in sham controls failed to disturb retention of the landmark navigation strategy. Because none of the PPC-injured animals were deficient in the landmark task, a result which is contrary to observations in other laboratories, the influence of post-surgical recovery interval upon acquisition of the landmark navigation strategy was explored. Animals were prepared with right PPC injuries and trained following either a 5 or 35 day recovery interval. Only those animals limited to the short recovery interval proved to have a spatial deficit in the landmark task. It is concluded that injuries in the PPC of either hemisphere disturb egocentric spatial functions. However, animals with left PPC injuries are able to compensate by using allocentric visual cues if they are available. It is due to the special role played by the right PPC in complex visuospatial functions that animals with this injury are unable to compensate.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号