首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   228篇
  免费   4篇
  国内免费   1篇
电工技术   2篇
综合类   1篇
化学工业   24篇
金属工艺   1篇
机械仪表   3篇
建筑科学   2篇
能源动力   19篇
轻工业   56篇
无线电   11篇
一般工业技术   8篇
冶金工业   75篇
原子能技术   1篇
自动化技术   30篇
  2023年   1篇
  2022年   3篇
  2021年   1篇
  2019年   2篇
  2017年   2篇
  2016年   9篇
  2014年   3篇
  2013年   10篇
  2012年   9篇
  2011年   18篇
  2010年   18篇
  2009年   10篇
  2008年   11篇
  2007年   10篇
  2006年   4篇
  2005年   7篇
  2004年   3篇
  2003年   7篇
  2002年   6篇
  2001年   1篇
  2000年   11篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   5篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   6篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   2篇
  1975年   3篇
  1973年   2篇
  1971年   1篇
  1965年   1篇
  1927年   1篇
排序方式: 共有233条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
11.
Inulin fructotransferase (IFTase; EC 4.2.2.18) has received great attention mainly due to its application in producing difructose anhydride III (DFA III), which is a novel functional sweetener. The object of this study was to investigate the induction of IFTase in Arthrobacter aurescens SK 8.001 with various carbon sources, especially inulin and DFA III. IFTase production could be significantly promoted by the supplement of inulin (5–50 g/L) and DFA III (5–20 g/L). Inulin at high initial concentrations gave no indication of catabolite repression, whereas 30 and 40 g/L DFA III intensely inhibited cell growth and IFTase activity. No fructose was detected in broth throughout the cultivation with inulin, and inulin was converted into DFA III and minor fructooligosaccharides. And when DFA III was the carbon source, DFA III was the only sugar detected in the broth. In conclusion, both DFA III and inulin are effective for IFTase induction, and inulin with higher IFTase activity proved to be a more potent inducer.  相似文献   
12.
Antimicrobial activity of nobiletin and tangeretin against Pseudomonas   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The inhibitory activities of two polymethoxylated flavone (PMFs) monomers (nobiletin and tangeretin) have been investigated against Pseudomonas fluorescens and Pseudomonas aeruginosain vitro. The effects on cell morphology, the release of cell constituents, the synthesis of proteins and the activities of key dehydrogenase were examined to elucidate their antibacterial mechanism. The concentration of transaminase and reducing sugar in bacterial solutions increased significantly when treated with nobiletin and tangeretin. Electron microscopy showed that the structure of the bacterial cells was destroyed and accompanied with induced cells plasmolysis. Nobiletin and tangeretin also inhibited the activities of succinate dehydrogenase (SDH) and malate dehydrogenase (MDH), and reduced proteins synthesis in bacterial cells. It is proposed that nobiletin and tangeretin destroyed the permeability of the cell membrane, with release of the cell constituents, leading to metabolic dysfunction, inhibition of protein synthesis, and eventually to cell pyknosis and death.  相似文献   
13.
Two experiments are reported that use patients with visual extinction to examine how visual attention is influenced by action information in images. In Experiment 1 patients saw images of objects that were either correctly or incorrectly colocated for action, with the objects held by hands that were congruent or incongruent with those used premorbidly by the patients. The images were also shown from a 1st- and 3rd-person perspective. There was an overall reduction in extinction for objects colocated for action. In addition, there was an extra benefit when the objects were held in hands congruent with those used by the patients and when the objects were seen from a 1st-person perspective. This last result fits with an effect of motor simulation, over and above a purely visual effect based on positioning objects correctly for action. Experiment 2 showed that effects of hand congruence could emerge with images depicted from a 3rd-person perspective when patients saw themselves holding the objects. The data indicate 2 effects of action information on extinction: (a) an effect of colocating objects for action, which does not depend on a self-reference frame (a visual effect), and (b) an effect sensitive to object–hand congruence, which does depend on a self-reference frame (a motor-based effect). The self-reference frame is induced when stimuli are viewed from a 1st-person perspective and when an image of the self is seen from a 3rd-person perspective. Both visual and motor-based effects of action information facilitate the spread of attention across objects. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
14.
Aqueous solutions of gellan gum with comparable molecular mass but with different acyl contents were investigated by atomic force microscopy and rheological measurements in the presence of sodium chloride (NaCl). Results obtained were discussed in relation to our previous report using potassium chloride (KCl) as an added salt. For a low-acyl sample, continuous fibrous network structures were identified microscopically as in the case of KCl. The network structures were more heterogeneous than those formed with KCl in terms of the height distribution of molecular assemblies. Rheological thermal hysteresis between sol–gel transitions was detected as in the case of KCl. The storage modulus (G′) of the gelled system was ca. 15% of the corresponding data with KCl at 20 °C. For a high-acyl sample, no continuous network structures were identified but branches with observable ends were identified as in the case of KCl. The hysteresis was less evident than the corresponding data with KCl and for the low-acyl sample with NaCl. Also, G′ values at 20 °C were ca. 30% and 20% of the corresponding data with KCl and for the low-acyl sample with NaCl, respectively. Continuousness and homogeneity of network structures related to the hysteresis and elasticity of the system, respectively.  相似文献   
15.
16.
A study was conducted to investigate the body movements of participants waiting to be interviewed in one of two conditions: preparing to answer questions truthfully or preparing to lie. The effects of increased self-awareness were also investigated, with half of the participants facing a mirror; the other half facing a blank wall. Analysis of covertly obtained video footage showed a significant interaction for the duration of hand/arm movements between deception level and self-awareness. Without a mirror, participants expecting to lie spent less time moving their hands than those expecting to tell the truth; the opposite was seen in the presence of a mirror. Participants expecting to lie also had higher levels of anxiety and thought that they were left waiting for less time than those expecting to tell the truth. These findings led to the identification of further research areas with the potential to support deception detection in security applications.  相似文献   
17.
[Correction Notice: An erratum for this article was reported in Vol 13(3) of Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes (see record 2008-10750-001). The last sentence in the second paragraph of the Discussion on page 128 should read as follows: "A second possibility is that in the marked ITI group, the marking of irrelevant events in the middle of the intertrial interval promoted associations between those events and food, which then interfered with the learning of an association between SI and food."] In four experiments we investigated pigeons' acquisition of a successive discrimination with a trace autoshaping procedure. The conditioned stimuli were 5-s presentations of colored key lights, one of which was followed by food after a 5-s delay. In Experiment 1, which used spatially defined cues, we found that acquisition of differential responding to the reinforced cue was facilitated when a brief flash of light immediately followed both reinforced and nonreinforced cues. Experiment 2 found a similar enhancement by the added light flash in a purely visual discrimination. Experiment 3 found that the flash facilitated learning only when presented immediately after the discriminative cues, and not when it occurred immediately before the cues or at the time of reinforcement. A fourth experiment found this facilitation effect only when the flash and reinforcement occurred on the same trial. These results are interpreted in terms of marking: The flash enhanced learning because it triggered a backward scan through recent memory to search for possible predictors. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
18.
Recent evidence has shown that inhibition of return, IOR, is impaired in patients with parietal damage with or without clinical signs of neglect (Bartolomeo, Sieroff, Decaix, & Chokron, 2001; Vivas, Humphreys, & Fuentes, 2003, respectively). In addition to environment-based IOR, Tipper et al. (1991) showed that IOR could be also associated with dynamic, object-based representations. In our study, we examined four patients with unilateral lesions to the parietal lobe, and a group of healthy controls, in an IOR procedure with moving objects where a pre-cued object could move, clockwise or counterclockwise, 90° in polar coordinates. The group of control participants showed a small but significant object-based IOR effect. In contrast, the patients showed an object-based IOR effect when the objects moved from the contralesional field toward the ipsilesional field, whereas there was no IOR effect when they moved from the ipsilesional to the contralesional field. These findings are discussed in terms of the role of the parietal cortex in implementing attentional biases in both environment-based (Vivas et al., 2003) and object-based frames of reference. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
19.
The tetrasaccharide 4, a substructure of ganglioside GQ1balpha, shows a remarkable affinity for the myelin-associated glycoprotein (MAG) and was therefore selected as starting point for a lead optimization program. In our search for structurally simplified and pharmacokinetically improved mimics of 4, antagonists with modifications of the core disaccharide Galbeta(1-3)GalNAc, as well as the terminal alpha(2-3)- and the internal alpha(2-6)-linked neuraminic acid were synthesized and tested in target-based binding assays. Compared to the reference tetrasaccharide 4, the most potent antagonist 17 exhibits a 360-fold improved affinity. Furthermore, pharmacokinetic parameters such as stability in the cerebrospinal fluid, logD and permeation through the BBB indicate the drug-like properties of antagonist 17.  相似文献   
20.
The tissue structures of 2-year-old konjac tubers were investigated using a variable pressure scanning electron microscope equipped with an energy-dispersive X-ray spectrometer and an optical microscope. Konjac mannan (KM) cells are distributed all over the tuber and the spaces between the KM cells are filled with parenchyma. The KM cells are covered with scale-like cell walls and the size of the cells varies from 160 to 650 μm. Other kinds of particles with different sizes are observed in the honeycombed cells around the KM cells. The small grains relate to protein granules and the spherical particles (~12 μm in diameter), which are composed of small granules (1–3 μm in diameter), are starch. The tuber also contains potassium and calcium in the parenchyma. Most calcium is found in the needle-like crystals in the form of calcium oxalate. Such crystals are also found in 1 year and seed tubers.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号