全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1459篇 |
免费 | 25篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
电工技术 | 5篇 |
综合类 | 1篇 |
化学工业 | 193篇 |
金属工艺 | 7篇 |
机械仪表 | 11篇 |
建筑科学 | 42篇 |
矿业工程 | 7篇 |
能源动力 | 23篇 |
轻工业 | 276篇 |
水利工程 | 5篇 |
石油天然气 | 8篇 |
无线电 | 64篇 |
一般工业技术 | 107篇 |
冶金工业 | 583篇 |
原子能技术 | 1篇 |
自动化技术 | 153篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 22篇 |
2020年 | 17篇 |
2019年 | 17篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 16篇 |
2015年 | 15篇 |
2014年 | 19篇 |
2013年 | 58篇 |
2012年 | 37篇 |
2011年 | 57篇 |
2010年 | 56篇 |
2009年 | 35篇 |
2008年 | 58篇 |
2007年 | 72篇 |
2006年 | 62篇 |
2005年 | 62篇 |
2004年 | 44篇 |
2003年 | 51篇 |
2002年 | 42篇 |
2001年 | 31篇 |
2000年 | 30篇 |
1999年 | 32篇 |
1998年 | 36篇 |
1997年 | 37篇 |
1996年 | 36篇 |
1995年 | 30篇 |
1994年 | 32篇 |
1993年 | 30篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 31篇 |
1989年 | 32篇 |
1988年 | 25篇 |
1987年 | 25篇 |
1986年 | 37篇 |
1985年 | 22篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 30篇 |
1982年 | 23篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1978年 | 19篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1964年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有1486条查询结果,搜索用时 145 毫秒
101.
E-ARK (European Archiving of Records and Knowledge Preservation) is currently introducing standardization, new tools, and infrastructure into pan-European digital archival systems. The results of this pilot project can be applied to higher education where research data centers are required to preserve data for re-use in accordance with new open policies.We elucidate how the E-ARK best practice survey has informed the development of new information packages for ingest, preservation, and re-use, and show how these can be applied to the Arts, Humanities, and Social Sciences with practical exemplars such as census data. 相似文献
102.
103.
Andrew Ireland Bill J. Ellis Andrew Cook Roderick Chapman Janet Barnes 《Journal of Automated Reasoning》2006,36(4):379-410
Using automated reasoning techniques, we tackle the niche activity of proving that a program is free from run-time exceptions.
Such a property is particularly valuable in high integrity software, for example, safety- or security-critical applications.
The context for our work is the SPARK Approach for the development of high integrity software. The SPARK Approach provides
a significant degree of automation in proving exception freedom. Where this automation fails, however, the programmer is burdened
with the task of interactively constructing a proof and possibly also having to supply auxiliary program annotations. We minimize
this burden by increasing the automation, through an integration of proof planning and a program analysis oracle. We advocate
a ‘cooperative’ integration, where proof-failure analysis directly constrains the search for auxiliary program annotations.
The approach has been successfully tested on industrial data. 相似文献
104.
105.
Janet Finlay 《Applied Artificial Intelligence》2013,27(7-8):633-651
One problem facing designers of interactive systems is catering to the wide range of users who will use a particular application. Understanding the user is critical to designing a usable interface. There are a number of ways of addressing this problem, including improved design methodologies using ''intuitive'' interface styles, adaptive interfaces, and better training and user support materials. In this article, we argue that each of these solutions involves pattern recognition in one form or another and that machine learning can therefore aid designers of interactive systems in these areas. We report on experiments that demonstrate the potential of machine learning to user modeling that has application to two of these areas in particular: adaptive systems and design methodologies. 相似文献
106.
Janet Soler 《Pedagogy, Culture & Society》1999,7(3):523-540
The late 1990s have seen a resurgence of the arguments for a skills-focused literacy instruction programme. This article explores the New Zealand-based public and professional debates surrounding the promotion of a technocratic approach to literacy instruction during the 1940s and 1950s, and the reoccurrence of the reading debate in late 1990s. In the early stages of both literacy crises, the professional competency of education officials and primary teachers was attacked by business interests and their political allies. The aftermath of the earlier crisis highlights the failure of this mechanistic and 'culture neutral' view of literacy instruction to cater for Maori children. The outcomes of the earlier debate signals a need avoid a similar conclusion to the current debate by adopting a broader approach to literacy instruction in the new millennium if language and literacy instruction programmes are to cater for complex cultural contexts, and the changing technological environments 相似文献
107.
Examined the power–dependency relationships between "whistle blowers" and their employers through the regression analyses of survey data from 636 employees of federal agencies who had reported evidence of official wrongdoing. Ss reported that they were more likely to suffer retaliation if they lacked the support of their supervisors and managers. Retaliation was also more severe if the wrongdoing was serious and if the S used channels outside the organization to report the incident. Following severe retaliation, Ss were likely to file official complaints about the retaliation and to leave their jobs involuntarily. Other predictors of reactions to retaliatory action are discussed. (36 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
108.
Maylor Elizabeth A.; Vousden Janet I.; Brown Gordon D. A. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1999,14(4):572
Age-related deficits in short-term memory have been widely reported, but reduced overall scores could reflect increased order errors, increased omissions, or increased intrusions. Different explanations for reduced short-term memory with aging lead to different predictions. In this study, young (n?=?68; M age?=?20 years) and older (n?=?99; M age?=?65 years) adults were presented with lists of letters and were asked to recall each list immediately in the correct order. Age differences in error patterns were similar for auditory and visual presentation. For example, older adults made more errors of every type, and a greater proportion of the older adults' errors were omissions. An additional condition, in which older adults were encouraged to guess, ruled out an age increase in response threshold as a full explanation for the results. The data were modeled by an oscillator-based computational model of memory for serial order. A good fit to the aging data was achieved by simultaneously altering two parameters that were interpreted as corresponding to frontal decline and response slowing. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
109.
Fifty-nine 3-year-olds were tested 3 times over a period of 7 months in order to assess the contribution of theory of mind to language development and of language to theory-of-mind development (including the independent contributions of syntax and semantics). Language competence was assessed with a standardized measure of reception and production of syntax and semantics (the Test of Early Language Development). Theory of mind was assessed with false-belief tasks and appearance–reality tasks. Earlier language abilities predicted later theory-of-mind test performance (controlling for earlier theory of mind), but earlier theory of mind did not predict later language test performance (controlling for earlier language). These findings are consistent with the argument that language is fundamental to theory-of mind development. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
110.
Sanders Raymond E.; Wise Janet L.; Liddle Cherie L.; Murphy Martin D. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1990,5(2):172
The usual superiority in frequency-of-occurrence judgments of younger vs older subjects was hypothesized to result from greater strategic encoding of the materials conveying frequency information. A subject-paced, visual search task was designed to control nontarget word encoding. Relative frequency judgments for the nontarget word pairs were equally accurate for younger and older subjects, and performance of both groups was above chance. Results suggest that strategic cover-task encoding can induce age differences in incidental frequency processing. Consistent with a nonoptimal as opposed to an optimal view, automatic processes require only minimal capacity for above-chance performance, but additional strategic resources can increase performance. When such additional resources are used more by younger than by older subjects, the former are better in frequency performance. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献