首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1009篇
  免费   20篇
  国内免费   1篇
电工技术   18篇
化学工业   180篇
金属工艺   6篇
机械仪表   19篇
建筑科学   27篇
能源动力   10篇
轻工业   31篇
石油天然气   7篇
无线电   113篇
一般工业技术   133篇
冶金工业   233篇
原子能技术   7篇
自动化技术   246篇
  2022年   6篇
  2021年   6篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   7篇
  2017年   12篇
  2016年   15篇
  2015年   10篇
  2014年   22篇
  2013年   37篇
  2012年   29篇
  2011年   34篇
  2010年   39篇
  2009年   40篇
  2008年   29篇
  2007年   43篇
  2006年   41篇
  2005年   31篇
  2004年   31篇
  2003年   23篇
  2002年   26篇
  2001年   25篇
  2000年   20篇
  1999年   16篇
  1998年   42篇
  1997年   35篇
  1996年   22篇
  1995年   22篇
  1994年   17篇
  1993年   25篇
  1992年   13篇
  1991年   12篇
  1990年   16篇
  1989年   14篇
  1988年   19篇
  1987年   17篇
  1986年   16篇
  1985年   23篇
  1984年   17篇
  1983年   15篇
  1982年   10篇
  1981年   17篇
  1980年   9篇
  1979年   12篇
  1978年   9篇
  1977年   9篇
  1976年   19篇
  1975年   7篇
  1974年   7篇
  1973年   7篇
  1967年   5篇
排序方式: 共有1030条查询结果,搜索用时 484 毫秒
101.
Changes in the principal’s role, resulting from the computerization of a high-school’s instruction-administration subsystem (i.e. a significant technological change), occurred in six major categories: accountability, instruction evaluation, supervision, feedback, frequency of meetings, and shared decision-making. The principal’s interrelations with department heads, homeroom teachers and teachers, tightened significantly. The principal’s interrelations with the school computer administrator (a new role created as a result of the introduction of a school management information system), were found to be tight and on a daily basis. The results of this research might have significant theoretical and practical implications. They should be related to SMIS impact on schooling in general and on the principal’s role in particular.  相似文献   
102.
Most previous work on authorship attribution has focused on the case in which we need to attribute an anonymous document to one of a small set of candidate authors. In this paper, we consider authorship attribution as found in the wild: the set of known candidates is extremely large (possibly many thousands) and might not even include the actual author. Moreover, the known texts and the anonymous texts might be of limited length. We show that even in these difficult cases, we can use similarity-based methods along with multiple randomized feature sets to achieve high precision. Moreover, we show the precise relationship between attribution precision and four parameters: the size of the candidate set, the quantity of known-text by the candidates, the length of the anonymous text and a certain robustness score associated with a attribution.  相似文献   
103.
Although three-dimensional (3-D) partial inductance modeling costs have decreased with stable, sparse approximations of the inductance matrix and its inverse, 3-D models are still intractable when applied to full chip timing or crosstalk analysis. The 3-D partial inductance matrix (or its inverse) is too large to be extracted or simulated when power-grid cross-sections are made wide to capture proximity effect and wires are discretized finely to capture skin effect. Fortunately, 3-D inductance models are unnecessary in VLSI interconnect analysis. Because return currents follow interconnect wires, long interconnect wires can be accurately modeled as two-dimensional (2-D) transmission lines and frequency-dependent loop impedances extracted using 2-D methods . Furthermore, this frequency dependence can be approximated with compact circuit models for both uncoupled and coupled lines. Three-dimensional inductance models are only necessary to handle worst case effects such as simultaneous switching in the end regions. This paper begins by explaining and defending the 2-D modeling approach. It then extends the extraction algorithm to efficiently include distant return paths. Finally, a novel synthesis technique is described that approximates the frequency-dependent series impedance of VLSI interconnects with compact circuit models suitable for timing and noise analysis.  相似文献   
104.
A predominant view among urban economists is that the exponential function is an accurate representation of the spatial distribution of population densities in urban areas but an inadequate representation of the spatial distribution of non-residential activities. In this paper we suggest that such a view is not warranted either on theoretical or empirical grounds. Discreditation of the exponential function for non-residential density is not based on valid statistical tests, which refute the function, but on scholars' impression of how good the function fits the data. It is suggested in this paper that proper tests of the functional form can be conducted using the generalized Box-Cox transformation function, which permits estimation and testing of many functional forms. Such testing is conducted using 1996 data on densities of total floor-space and industry floor-space for manufacturing, commerce and services from Tel Aviv-Yafo. The results obtained are illuminating in showing that, contrary to the popular view in the literature, the exponential function is an appropriate form for representing the spatial distribution of total non-residential densities in Tel Aviv-Yafo which cannot be dismissed on formal statistical grounds. As far as the individual industries are concerned we cannot reject the exponential form, at the 1% significance level, for services and commerce and it is rejected for manufacturing. Received: December 1998/Accepted: October 1999  相似文献   
105.
106.
The propagation of fronts and the emergence of spatiotemporal patterns on a cylindrically shaped thin catalytic reactor is simulated with a homogeneous model of a fixed catalytic bed, with characteristically large Lewis and Peclet numbers, and a first-order Arrhenius kinetics (i.e., thermokinetic model) which may be coupled with slow changes of catalytic activity (i.e., oscillatory kinetics). Planar fronts of the thermokinetic model may undergo symmetry breaking in the transversal direction only at relatively low Lewis number, but for high Le the front remains flat. Patterns due to oscillatory kinetics in reactors of high Le are shown, for the first time, to undergo symmetry breaking in the azimuthal direction when the perimeter is sufficiently large. The generic regular patterns simulated then are rotating multi-wave patterns of constant rotation-speed and oscillatory-‘firing’ ones, and theirs selection is highly sensitive to governing parameters and initial conditions. The results are organized in bifurcation diagrams showing the coexisting two-dimensional solutions with varying perimeter. Increasing convective velocity or reactor radius leads to symmetry breaking of regular patterns and the system may switch to chaos.  相似文献   
107.
This study compared memory for words and the font in which they appeared (or the voice speaking them) in young and old participants, to explore whether age-related differences in episodic word memory are due to age-related differences in memory for perceptual–contextual information. In each of 3 experiments, young and older participants were presented with words to learn. The words were presented in either 1 of 2 font types, or in 1 of 2 male voices, and participants paid attention either to the fonts or voices or to the meaning of the words. Participants were then tested on both word and font or voice memory. Results showed that younger participants had better explicit memory for font and voice memory and for the words themselves but that older participants benefited at least as much as younger people when perceptual characteristics of the words were reinstated. There was no evidence of an age-related impairment in the encoding of perceptual–contextual information. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
108.
[Correction Notice: An erratum for this article was reported in Vol 23(5) of Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition (see record 2008-09898-001). On page 854, two Hebrew words are missing from Appendix F. The corrected Appendix appears with the erratum.] All Hebrew words are composed of 2 interwoven morphemes: a triconsonantal root and a phonological word pattern. The lexical representations of these morphemic units were examined using masked priming. When primes and targets shared an identical word pattern, neither lexical decision nor naming of targets was facilitated. In contrast, root primes facilitated both lexical decisions and naming of target words that were derived from these roots. This priming effect proved to be independent of meaning similarity because no priming effects were found when primes and targets were semantically but not morphologically related. These results suggest that Hebrew roots are lexical units whereas word patterns are not. A working model of lexical organization in Hebrew is offered on the basis of these results. (A correction concerning this article appears in Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 1997, Vol 23(5), 1189–1191. On page 854 of the current issue, two Hebrew words are missing from Appendix F. The corrected Appendix appears in this correction.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
109.
This study further tested an associative-deficit hypothesis (ADH; M. Naveh-Benjamin, 2000), which attributes a substantial part of older adults' deficient episodic memory performance to their difficulty in merging unrelated attributes-units of an episode into a cohesive unit. First, the results of 2 experiments replicate those observed by M. Naveh-Benjamin (2000) showing that older adults are particularly deficient in memory tests requiring associations. Second, the results extend the type of stimuli (pictures) under which older adults show this associative deficit. Third, the results support an ADH in that older adults show less of an associative deficit when the components of the episodes used are already connected in memory, thereby facilitating their encoding and retrieval. Finally, a group of younger adults who encoded the information under divided-attention conditions did not show this associative deficit. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
110.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号