首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   9781篇
  免费   598篇
  国内免费   640篇
电工技术   159篇
技术理论   14篇
综合类   1731篇
化学工业   203篇
金属工艺   54篇
机械仪表   91篇
建筑科学   1690篇
矿业工程   191篇
能源动力   66篇
轻工业   310篇
水利工程   275篇
石油天然气   36篇
武器工业   3篇
无线电   435篇
一般工业技术   573篇
冶金工业   2921篇
自动化技术   2267篇
  2024年   34篇
  2023年   123篇
  2022年   196篇
  2021年   298篇
  2020年   347篇
  2019年   321篇
  2018年   319篇
  2017年   346篇
  2016年   388篇
  2015年   441篇
  2014年   657篇
  2013年   693篇
  2012年   497篇
  2011年   656篇
  2010年   601篇
  2009年   616篇
  2008年   561篇
  2007年   529篇
  2006年   549篇
  2005年   467篇
  2004年   406篇
  2003年   363篇
  2002年   300篇
  2001年   263篇
  2000年   102篇
  1999年   76篇
  1998年   39篇
  1997年   35篇
  1996年   19篇
  1994年   21篇
  1993年   21篇
  1992年   19篇
  1990年   25篇
  1989年   21篇
  1988年   27篇
  1987年   20篇
  1986年   18篇
  1966年   26篇
  1965年   28篇
  1964年   56篇
  1963年   46篇
  1962年   18篇
  1961年   43篇
  1960年   22篇
  1959年   30篇
  1958年   22篇
  1957年   33篇
  1956年   24篇
  1955年   47篇
  1954年   28篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 11 毫秒
41.
针对互联网中存在的恶意行为,特别是社交网络应用中的在线恶意行为,通常使用基于用户多维特征的聚类分析算法进行检测。提出一种动态特征选择算法(DFSA),使用具有特征加权熵的模糊C均值目标函数,首先为参数构建一个学习模式,自动计算每个特征权重,并剔除权重小于阈值的特征,动态选择重要的特征,迭代地更新隶属函数、簇中心和特征权重直到最优化为止,最后识别出具有高精度的恶意用户行为簇。仿真结果表明,对比SDAFS算法、ELAFC算法和NADMB算法,DFSA算法在Rand指数、Jaccard指数和归一化互信息量3个主要性能指标上均有改善。  相似文献   
42.
随着我国经济水平的不断提高,国家和人民对于生态环境的重视程度不断加强,生态文明建设已成为当前国家建设的重点内容.基于此,本文围绕生态文明建设中农村环境问题和对策展开探讨.  相似文献   
43.
A model of cultural dynamics based on the work of Colin Renfrew is briefly described. It is shown how the general concepts of complexity and error pertain to this kind of cultural model. In particular, it is suggested that conflict in cultural contexts arises through the generation of diverse simple models of the culture, created by different members of the culture. We indicate how general system-theoretic concepts can thus be brought to bear on problems of conflict in and between cultures.  相似文献   
44.
Mixed tenure is the predominant development and regeneration strategy and is a key component of UK housing and urban policy. It is purported to provide wide-ranging social, environmental and economic benefits to residents. While there is a large literature on mixed tenure, policy makers are likely to rely on reviews and summaries of the evidence rather than primary studies. But can they rely on such reviews? Using systematic review methods this paper critically appraises recent reviews for the evidence that mixed tenure policies and strategies have achieved any of these expected benefits. Of the six UK reviews of primary studies, most drew on less than half the available primary studies, none provided a critical appraisal of individual studies and made no comment on conflicting evidence between and within studies. While the reviews gave indications of the deficiencies of the evidence base, rather than focus on the implications of these deficiencies, four of the six reviews emphasised the positive effects of tenure mix.  相似文献   
45.
欧洲受到当今全球性生态和经济的挑战,可持续性和适应性的建筑结构设计概念再次变得愈发重要。此外,个性化的要求、人口结构的变化、民族的多样化以及向灵活工作方式的过渡也激发了对适应性住房需求的日益增长。自20世纪80年代末以来,灵活住宅的范围在欧洲显著扩大,并产生了多种类型:从功能开放的楼层平面、活动墙体的楼层平面以及各种组合类型,到前卫的空间场景、实验性高科技适应结构以及开放功能计划的建筑类型。建立起一种实现灵活性之方法的框架,并介绍了几个具有创新性的案例。  相似文献   
46.
A great deal of developmental research has relied on self-reports solicited using the “some/other” question format (“Some students think that… but other students think that…”). This article reports tests of the assumptions underlying its use: that it conveys to adolescents that socially undesirable attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors are not uncommon and legitimizes reporting them, yielding more valid self-reports than would be obtained by “direct” questions, which do not mention what other people think or do. A meta-analysis of 11 experiments embedded in four surveys of diverse samples of adolescents did not support the assumption that the some/other form increases validity. Although the some/other form led adolescents to think that undesirable attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors were more common and resulted in more reports of those attitudes and behaviors, answers to some/other questions were lower in criterion validity than were answers to direct questions. Because some/other questions take longer to ask and answer and require greater cognitive effort from participants (because they involve more words), and because they decrease measurement accuracy, the some/other question format seems best avoided. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
47.
Reports an error in Age differences in psychosocial predictors of positive and negative affect: A longitudinal investigation of young, midlife, and older adults by Tim D. Windsor and Kaarin J. Anstey (Psychology and Aging, 2010[Sep], Vol 25[3], 641-652). Contains an error in Figure 3, on page 649. The correction discusses where to find the correct data. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2010-18944-009.) Research has consistently shown that despite aging-related losses, older adults have high levels of emotional well-being relative to those in young and midlife adults. We aimed to contribute to knowledge around the factors that predict emotional well-being over the life course by examining age group differences in associations of positive and negative social exchanges and mastery beliefs with positive and negative affect in a sample of 7,472 young, midlife, and older adults assessed on 2 measurement occasions, 4 years apart. Results from structural equation models indicated lower levels of negative affect with advancing age. Mastery was consistently related to higher well-being, with the strongest associations evident for young adults. Older adults reported the most frequent positive and least frequent negative social exchanges; however, associations of social relations with affect tended to be stronger among young and midlife adults relative to older adults. Results are discussed in the context of life course perspectives on goal orientations and self-regulatory processes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
48.
In the present research, we test the assumption that emotional mimicry and contagion are moderated by group membership. We report two studies using facial electromyography (EMG; Study 1), Facial Action Coding System (FACS; Study 2), and self-reported emotions (Study 2) as dependent measures. As predicted, both studies show that ingroup anger and fear displays were mimicked to a greater extent than outgroup displays of these emotions. The self-report data in Study 2 further showed specific divergent reactions to outgroup anger and fear displays. Outgroup anger evoked fear, and outgroup fear evoked aversion. Interestingly, mimicry increased liking for ingroup models but not for outgroup models. The findings are discussed in terms of the social functions of emotions in group contexts. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
49.
Typically developing children understand and predict others' behavior by extracting and processing relevant information such as the logic of their actions within the situational constraints and the intentions conveyed by their gaze direction and emotional expressions. Children with autism have difficulties understanding and predicting others' actions. With the use of eye tracking and behavioral measures, we investigated action understanding mechanisms used by 18 children with autism and a well-matched group of 18 typically developing children. Results showed that children with autism (a) consider situational constraints in order to understand the logic of an agent's action and (b) show typical usage of the agent's emotional expressions to infer his or her intentions. We found (c) subtle atypicalities in the way children with autism respond to an agent's direct gaze and (d) marked impairments in their ability to attend to and interpret referential cues such as a head turn for understanding an agent's intentions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
50.
Many purely physical complex systems, in which there are both stochasticity and local interactions between the components, exhibit record dynamics. The temporal statistics of record dynamics is a Poisson process operating on a logarithmic rather than a linear time scale (i.e. a log-Poisson process). Record dynamics often drive substantial changes in complex systems when new high water marks in partially stochastic processes trigger new events. Social insect colonies are exemplary complex biological systems in which many of the local interactions of the components have been moulded by natural selection for the common good. Here, we combine experimental manipulation of ant colony demography with modelling to test the hypothesis that social interactions are the mechanism underlying the record dynamics. We found that compared with the control, log-Poisson statistics were disrupted in colonies in which the pattern of interactions was modified by the removal of the brood, and disappeared completely in ‘callow’ colonies composed entirely of very young workers from the same age cohort. We conclude that a subtle interplay between the demography of the society and the pattern of the interactions between the ants is crucial for the emergence of record dynamics. This could help identify what makes an ant colony a cohesive society.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号