首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   19134篇
  免费   1477篇
  国内免费   820篇
电工技术   1210篇
综合类   1893篇
化学工业   665篇
金属工艺   2103篇
机械仪表   4297篇
建筑科学   361篇
矿业工程   615篇
能源动力   213篇
轻工业   277篇
水利工程   77篇
石油天然气   1326篇
武器工业   270篇
无线电   3882篇
一般工业技术   1159篇
冶金工业   761篇
原子能技术   95篇
自动化技术   2227篇
  2024年   72篇
  2023年   258篇
  2022年   404篇
  2021年   481篇
  2020年   535篇
  2019年   399篇
  2018年   418篇
  2017年   598篇
  2016年   671篇
  2015年   722篇
  2014年   1081篇
  2013年   1054篇
  2012年   1385篇
  2011年   1345篇
  2010年   1012篇
  2009年   1057篇
  2008年   995篇
  2007年   1343篇
  2006年   1152篇
  2005年   1063篇
  2004年   897篇
  2003年   849篇
  2002年   658篇
  2001年   586篇
  2000年   473篇
  1999年   380篇
  1998年   287篇
  1997年   250篇
  1996年   232篇
  1995年   193篇
  1994年   148篇
  1993年   85篇
  1992年   85篇
  1991年   64篇
  1990年   49篇
  1989年   37篇
  1988年   26篇
  1987年   15篇
  1986年   10篇
  1985年   9篇
  1984年   11篇
  1983年   7篇
  1982年   6篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   4篇
  1965年   3篇
  1964年   3篇
  1959年   2篇
  1958年   2篇
  1955年   2篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
71.
This paper presents the development of an optimization and a computer-simulation model to evaluate the process plans of a manufacturing center by analyzing the effect of tool failure on system performance. The GPSS/PC simulation program that is used in this study has been built with variables, functions and matrices so that many experiments could be conducted with the model. Sensitivity analysis is presented and the developed model has proven to be useful in determining optimum sequencing of parts for various operating policies.  相似文献   
72.
The magnitude of the Stroop effect is known to be modulated by the proportion of trials on which the irrelevant word and relevant ink color correspond. This has often been attributed to a conscious strategy of increased (or decreased) reliance on the irrelevant words when these are more likely (or less likely) to correspond to the ink colors. However, the present data from a Stroop-like task involving successively presented arrows indicate instead that this type of modulation can be automatic because it can occur even if the irrelevant stimuli are not phenomenally visible. In this case participants cannot determine the proportion of compatible trials to direct their strategy. An automatic, item-specific associative interpretation can account for these findings. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
73.
Two experiments examined how cross-list directional associations influenced list-method directed forgetting and the degree of interference observed on each list. Each List 1 item had a (a) bidirectionally related item on List 2 (chip ←→ potato), (b) forward association with an item on List 2 (chip → wood), (c) backward association from an item on List 2 (chip ← chisel), or (d) no relationship with List 2 items. The results revealed that associative relationships that eliminated retroactive interference in the baseline condition also eliminated the directed forgetting costs. In contrast, associative relationships did not affect List 2 recall in the forget group, which remained unchanged across experimental conditions. However, certain conditions reduced proactive interference in the remember group, thereby eliminating the benefits of directed forgetting. The directed forgetting costs and benefits were observed independently of each other. The authors propose that these effects emerged from a combination of item and context strengthening induced by different associative directions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
74.
When humans plan to execute a tool-use action, they can only specify the bodily movement parameters by taking into account the external target or goal of the tool-use action and the target-movement mapping implemented by the tool. In this study, the authors used the movement precuing method to investigate how people prepare for actions made with tools. More specifically, they asked whether people would be able to specify the spatial target and the target-movement mapping of the tool-use action independently of each other, and to what degree they would be able to prepare these components in advance. In 3 experiments, they precued either the target or the target-movement mapping of tool-use actions involving either a compatible or an incompatible target-movement mapping. Results indicate that participants benefit from partial advance information about the target-movement mapping, whereas no significant effects were found for precuing the spatial target of the action. These results occurred regardless of whether the target-movement mapping was compatible or incompatible and provide evidence for the notion that the target-movement mapping of a tool-use action is part of its cognitive representation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
75.
For 30 years, A. G. Greenwald and H. G. Shulman's (1973) psychological refractory period (PRP) study has been cited as evidence for perfect timesharing with ideomotor (IM)-compatible tasks. Recently, M.-C. Lien, R. W. Proctor, and P. A. Allen (2002) failed to replicate these results and concluded that IM compatibility is neither necessary nor sufficient to eliminate the PRP effect. A. G. Greenwald (2003) attributed Lien et al.'s nonreplication to the use of (a) a non-IM-compatible task, (b) varied trial spacing, and/or (c) inappropriate instructions. The authors of the present article argue that the first 2 factors are not critical and that instructions merely affect the criterion for speed versus accuracy. In each of Greenwald's experiments, dual-task costs were evident on response time or error rates. Furthermore, the small dual-task costs in his study are consistent with a bottleneck model. Thus, Greenwald (2003) does not provide evidence that IM-compatible tasks enable perfect timesharing. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
76.
The technological properties of the hot-work tool steels depend on their microstructural quality induced by the production process and the heat treatment. One of the frequently applied methods to improve microstructure is preheat treatment. The treatment of carbonitriding X37CrMoV5-1 steel has been investigated in fluidized bed furnaces. Thickness and phase composition have been conducted using optical microscopy Neophot 32 and scanning microscopy, JEOL 5400, after etching in nital.  相似文献   
77.
It has been reported that it is harder to switch to a strong, well-practiced task from a weaker, less-practiced task than vice versa. Three experiments replicated this surprising asymmetry and investigated how it is affected by a reduction in interference between tasks. Experiment 1 progressively delayed the onset of the stimulus attribute associated with the stronger task. Experiments 2 and 3 separated the response sets of the tasks. Both manipulations reduced, without eliminating, interference of the stronger with the weaker task but reversed the asymmetry of switch costs, resulting in a larger cost of switching to the weaker task. The results are interpreted in terms of a model of the interactions between control input, task strength, and task priming. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
78.
潘静 《冶金动力》2003,(5):20-21,27
从理论上分析了产生地电流干扰的原因,从而在实践上提出了一些经常被忽略,却是行之有效的方法来减小地电流的干扰,设计出高质量的电路。  相似文献   
79.
小型微/纳米级三坐标测量机的研制   总被引:11,自引:1,他引:10  
介绍一套低成本小型三维坐标测量机的研制过程,作为微/纳米级超精密测量设备,整体机台结构采用花岗岩材料;X/Y轴的移动平台利用压电材料摩擦驱动与挠性变形原理,长短两行程可达25mm范围及5nm的微位移,藉自行研发的衍射干涉激光尺,位置回馈测量系统可具有0.025nm的分辨率;测量探头在一般DVD光读取头的自动聚焦原理基础上改装,最佳分辨率可达2.5nm,该雏形机适于测量微机电组件、微型器件及膜厚等。  相似文献   
80.
王忠生  邹强 《钻采工艺》2006,29(6):19-20
在川渝地区,钻定向井、水平井必然要面对地层岩性变化大、硬度高等复杂地质情况,常常会遇到因井下复杂、事故、修改地质目标等原因需要填井侧钻。侧钻的方法不同,侧钻的成功率和效率差异很大。文中分析了川渝地区近几年硬地层定向井、水平井侧钻效率不高、有时甚至失败的原因,归纳总结了近几年常用的几种侧钻方法的施工效果,提出要根据具体条件灵活应用这几种侧钻方法。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号