首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4859篇
  免费   376篇
  国内免费   172篇
电工技术   59篇
综合类   160篇
化学工业   1821篇
金属工艺   663篇
机械仪表   36篇
建筑科学   120篇
矿业工程   62篇
能源动力   88篇
轻工业   1070篇
水利工程   25篇
石油天然气   443篇
武器工业   3篇
无线电   67篇
一般工业技术   245篇
冶金工业   449篇
原子能技术   15篇
自动化技术   81篇
  2024年   22篇
  2023年   71篇
  2022年   185篇
  2021年   206篇
  2020年   149篇
  2019年   138篇
  2018年   127篇
  2017年   163篇
  2016年   191篇
  2015年   178篇
  2014年   234篇
  2013年   283篇
  2012年   301篇
  2011年   410篇
  2010年   281篇
  2009年   284篇
  2008年   235篇
  2007年   274篇
  2006年   276篇
  2005年   227篇
  2004年   188篇
  2003年   176篇
  2002年   126篇
  2001年   124篇
  2000年   103篇
  1999年   55篇
  1998年   60篇
  1997年   40篇
  1996年   36篇
  1995年   23篇
  1994年   34篇
  1993年   33篇
  1992年   26篇
  1991年   20篇
  1990年   23篇
  1989年   13篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   12篇
  1986年   7篇
  1985年   15篇
  1984年   13篇
  1983年   5篇
  1981年   4篇
  1977年   3篇
  1964年   3篇
  1963年   2篇
  1961年   2篇
  1960年   2篇
  1958年   3篇
  1955年   2篇
排序方式: 共有5407条查询结果,搜索用时 13 毫秒
91.
采用对峙培养法,检测分离于土壤中的链霉菌TD-1代谢产物对饲料中污染霉菌构巢曲霉(Aspergillus nidulans)、纯绿青霉(Penicillium verrucosum)、赭曲霉(Aspergillus ochraceus)、黄曲霉(Aspergillus flavus)、黑曲霉(Aspergillus niger)、米曲霉(Aspergillus oryzae)、茄腐镰刀菌(Fusarium solani)、桔青霉(Penicillum citrinum)、串珠镰刀菌(Fusarium moniliforme)的抑菌活性,结果表明:TD-1菌株代谢产物对以上霉菌具有较强的抑制作用,抑菌带宽度为10 mm左右.通过TD-1菌株的代谢过程曲线发现,该菌株发酵至第6 d抑菌率达90%左右.稳定性试验结果显示:该菌株所产生的活性物质具有较好的耐热性、耐酸碱性、抗紫外辐射能力.  相似文献   
92.
通过荧光光谱法和碳酸钙沉积法研究了荧光标记复配水处理剂的性能并进行了评价。试验表明,该荧光标记复配水处理剂的荧光强度与其质量浓度呈良好的线性关系,且荧光示踪剂对复配水处理剂的阻垢性能基本没有影响。同时考察了温度、时间、pH等因素对荧光标记复配水处理剂荧光强度的影响。结果表明,荧光标记复配水处理剂的稳定性良好,受上述情况的影响较小。因此通过对荧光标记复配水处理剂的荧光强度分析可以确定水处理系统内复配水处理剂的含量。将荧光发射值与此复配水处理剂的浓度关联起来,从而定量地控制循环冷却水中复配水处理剂的有效浓度。  相似文献   
93.
杨帆 《乙烯工业》2002,14(4):58-60
研究了复合型缓蚀剂SM2001对45#碳钢在模拟工艺水中的缓蚀性能,原子吸收法测定冷却水中溶出Fe^2 量及极化曲线测试结果表明,SM2001对45#碳钢具有良好的腐蚀性能,在金属表面形成性能良好的缓蚀膜。  相似文献   
94.
The interaction of bacteria and metal surfaces   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
This review discusses different examples for the interaction of bacteria and metal surfaces based on work reported previously by various authors and work performed by the author with colleagues at other institutions and with his graduate students at CEEL. Traditionally it has been assumed that the interaction of bacteria with metal surfaces always causes increased corrosion rates (“microbiologically influenced corrosion” (MIC)). However, more recently it has been observed that many bacteria can reduce corrosion rates of different metals and alloys in many corrosive environments. For example, it has been found that certain strains of Shewanella can prevent pitting of Al 2024 in artificial seawater, tarnishing of brass and rusting of mild steel. It has been observed that corrosion started again when the biofilm was killed by adding antibiotics. The mechanism of corrosion protection seems to be different for different bacteria since it has been found that the corrosion potential Ecorr became more negative in the presence of Shewanella ana and algae, but more positive in the presence of Bacillus subtilis. These findings have been used in an initial study of the bacterial battery in which Shewanella oneidensis MR-1 was added to a cell containing Al 2024 and Cu in a growth medium. It was found that the power output of this cell continuously increased with time. In the microbial fuel cell (MFC) bacteria oxidize the fuel and transfer electrons directly to the anode. In initial studies EIS has been used to characterize the anode, cathode and membrane properties for different operating conditions of a MFC that contained Shewanella oneidensis MR-1. Cell voltage (V)—current density (i) curves were obtained using potentiodynamic sweeps. The current output of a MFC has been monitored for different experimental conditions.  相似文献   
95.
以三氯硫磷为原料,与正丁胺进行亲核取代反应合成中间体正丁基硫代磷酰二氯,再将正丁基硫代磷酰二氯与液氨通过氨解反应合成正丁基硫代磷酰三胺,商品名为Nitrogeny.反应中产生的HCl用三乙胺来吸收.探讨了各种反应因素如反应物的配比,三乙胺的用量,反应时间,温度等因素对抑制剂活性的影响,通过正交实验确定了正丁基硫代磷酰三胺的最佳工艺合成反应条件:n(RNH2):n(PSCl3)=1:1.2;三乙胺17.5 mL,液氨80 g;反应温度θ1=10 ℃,θ2=0 ℃;反应时间t1=3.5 h,t2=60 min.此时抑制活性达到最大值92.50%.  相似文献   
96.
In list-method directed forgetting, participants are cued to intentionally forget a previously studied list while remembering a subsequently presented 2nd list. Results from prior research are inconclusive on whether older adults show deficits in this type of task. In 3 experiments, the authors reexamined the issue and compared younger and older adults' responsiveness to the forget cue. Across the experiments, the forget cue was varied within and between participants, the 2 lists were unrelated and related to each other, and recall of the lists was required simultaneously and successively. In none of the 3 experiments did any age-related difference in directed forgetting performance emerge. List-method directed forgetting is assumed to reflect retrieval inhibition. The present results thus challenge the proposal of a general inhibitory deficit in older adults' memory performance. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
97.
Rat strain differences in the acoustic startle response (ASR) and prepulse inhibition (PPI) of that response are of increasing interest, especially as the genetics of PPI may provide an approach to studying the genetics of certain mental illnesses. However, strain differences in PPI are confounded by differences in ASR. To clarify this issue, the authors investigated the ASR and PPI across a range of startling stimulus intensities (70 dB-120 dB) in Wistar and Sprague-Dawley rats (N=96). Sprague-Dawleys showed more PPI of ASR capacity (response limit) than Wistars. In contrast, Wistars exhibited greater PPI than Sprague-Dawleys, as measured by an increase in response threshold. This dissociation suggests that PPI is more complex than that assessed by single startling stimulus intensity. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
98.
Inhibition of return (IOR) refers to slower responding to a stimulus that is presented at the same, rather than a different location as a preceding, spatially nonpredictive, stimulus. Repetition priming refers to speeded responding to a stimulus that duplicates the visual characteristics of a stimulus that precedes it. IOR and repetition priming effects interact in nonspatial discrimination tasks but not in localization tasks; three experiments examined whether this is due to processing differences or due to response differences between tasks. Two stimuli, S1 and S2, occurred on each trial. In Experiment 1, S1 and S2 were both peripheral arrows; in Experiment 2, S1 was a central arrow and S2 was a peripheral nondirectional rectangle; in Experiment 3, S1 was a peripheral nondirectional rectangle and S2 was a peripheral arrow. S1 never required a response; S2 required a localization or a discrimination response. Despite evidence that form information was likely extracted from the arrow stimuli, the localization task revealed no repetition priming: IOR occurred regardless of shared visual identity of the S1 and S2 arrows. The discrimination task revealed IOR only when the visual identity changed from S1 to S2; otherwise, facilitation occurred. These results suggest that IOR is masked by repetition priming only when the response depends on the explicit processing of form information; repetition priming does not occur when such information is extracted automatically but is task (and response) irrelevant. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
99.
This study investigated age differences in the ability to suppress and amplify expressive behavior during emotional arousal. Young and old participants viewed 3 film clips about medical procedures while their behavioral, autonomic, and subjective responses were recorded. Half of the participants viewed all 3 films without additional instructions; the other half was asked to suppress and amplify their behavioral expression during the 2nd and 3rd films. Except for heart rate, suppression and amplification produced similar patterns of autonomic activation. Neither suppression nor amplification had effects on self-reported emotion. There were no age differences in the ability to suppress or amplify emotional expression or in their physiological or subjective consequences. Considering that older people's unregulated reactivity was lower than that of young adults, suppression may have been easier and amplification more difficult for older adults. Voluntary emotion regulation might be one domain of human performance that is spared from age-related losses. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
100.
Visual search has been studied extensively, yet little is known about how its constituent processes affect subsequent emotional evaluation of searched-for and searched-through items. In 3 experiments, the authors asked observers to locate a colored pattern or tinted face in an array of other patterns or faces. Shortly thereafter, either the target or a distractor was rated on an emotional scale (patterns, cheerfulness; faces, trustworthiness). In general, distractors were rated more negatively than targets. Moreover, distractors presented near the target during search were rated significantly more negatively than those presented far from the target. Target-distractor proximity affected distractor ratings following both simple-feature and difficult-conjunction search, even when items appeared at different locations during evaluation than during search and when faces previously tinted during search were presented in grayscale at evaluation. An attentional inhibition account is offered to explain these effects of attention on emotional evaluation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号