首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1068篇
  免费   112篇
  国内免费   73篇
电工技术   24篇
综合类   155篇
化学工业   68篇
金属工艺   9篇
机械仪表   30篇
建筑科学   63篇
矿业工程   19篇
能源动力   15篇
轻工业   20篇
水利工程   31篇
石油天然气   10篇
武器工业   25篇
无线电   102篇
一般工业技术   124篇
冶金工业   253篇
原子能技术   6篇
自动化技术   299篇
  2023年   11篇
  2022年   18篇
  2021年   31篇
  2020年   23篇
  2019年   34篇
  2018年   24篇
  2017年   31篇
  2016年   40篇
  2015年   39篇
  2014年   72篇
  2013年   71篇
  2012年   66篇
  2011年   77篇
  2010年   65篇
  2009年   56篇
  2008年   50篇
  2007年   69篇
  2006年   60篇
  2005年   57篇
  2004年   66篇
  2003年   55篇
  2002年   36篇
  2001年   31篇
  2000年   18篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   11篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   9篇
  1995年   12篇
  1994年   9篇
  1993年   9篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   6篇
  1988年   3篇
  1986年   2篇
  1982年   6篇
  1967年   2篇
  1966年   4篇
  1965年   4篇
  1964年   5篇
  1963年   5篇
  1962年   3篇
  1961年   4篇
  1960年   4篇
  1959年   3篇
  1958年   3篇
  1957年   2篇
  1955年   4篇
排序方式: 共有1253条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
针对Matlab 6.5统计工具箱没有优化正态总体方差的区间估计,且没有给出正态总体方差检验的问题,通过编写Matlab程序,优化统计工具箱对正态总体方差的区间估计,开发正态总体方差的假设检验算法.实例表明该方法在推断统计方面实用性较强.  相似文献   
72.
一种基于小波变换的序列图像中小目标检测与跟踪算法   总被引:8,自引:0,他引:8  
该文提出了一种快速检测序列图像中低信噪比(2dB)小目标的算法。采用小波滤波器对每帧图像滤波,提高目标的信噪比,降低了低信噪比小目标检测算法的运算量;采用固定长度的假设检验算法,生成起始航迹,有效地避免航迹的漏检;采用截断序贯似然比检验,形成确认航迹。仿真结果表明应用此算法,可快速实现信噪比小于2dB的小目标(22)的检测与跟踪。  相似文献   
73.
针对导弹脱靶量的检验问题,分析了Bayes假设检验方法及其应用,推导了Bayes序贯检验方法.通过计算分析,证明通过利用验前信息,可以降低试验风险和减少试验子样,使鉴定结论更加可信.  相似文献   
74.
文中提出了将图像流分析与多级假设检验算法相结合的红外序列图像中运动点目标的检测方法。该方法利用目标的运动特性来实现检测算法.是一种快速简单的有效方法。  相似文献   
75.
选取长江上游8个主要水文站(直门达、石鼓、屏山、高场、北碚、寸滩、武隆、宜昌)控制流域作为子流域,利用其1979—2015年径流及气象数据,采用机器学习方法计算径流对降水与气温变化的敏感性,并与多元线性回归法和Budyko框架法进行对比,分析降水与气温变化对径流变化的贡献。结果表明:1979—2015年8个子流域径流与降水变化趋势整体不显著(p=0.01),气温呈显著上升趋势(p<0.01);径流对降水、气温的敏感性系数均值范围分别为0.37~0.76 mm/mm、-4.77~-33.53 mm/℃;径流对降水、气温的敏感性系数随着干旱指数的增大而减小;在干旱指数大的子流域降水变化对径流变化的影响更大,在干旱指数小的子流域气温变化对径流变化的影响更大。  相似文献   
76.
This study tested a dynamic field theory (DFT) of spatial working memory and an associated spatial precision hypothesis (SPH). Between 3 and 6 years of age, there is a qualitative shift in how children use reference axes to remember locations: 3-year-olds’ spatial recall responses are biased toward reference axes after short memory delays, whereas 6-year-olds’ responses are biased away from reference axes. According to the DFT and the SPH, quantitative improvements over development in the precision of excitatory and inhibitory working memory processes lead to this qualitative shift. Simulations of the DFT in Experiment 1 predict that improvements in precision should cause the spatial range of targets attracted toward a reference axis to narrow gradually over development, with repulsion emerging and gradually increasing until responses to most targets show biases away from the axis. Results from Experiment 2 with 3- to 5-year-olds support these predictions. Simulations of the DFT in Experiment 3 quantitatively fit the empirical results and offer insights into the neural processes underlying this developmental change. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
77.
This article presents a potential synthesis between the fitness indicator and life history models of human intelligence through consideration of the phenomena of ability differentiation and integration. The cognitive differentiation-integration effort hypothesis proposes that these effects result from a life history tradeoff between cognitive integration effort, a mating effort component associated with strengthening the positive manifold amongst abilities; and cognitive differentiation effort, a somatic effort component associated with the cultivation of specific abilities. This represents one of two largely independent sources of genetic variance in intelligence; the other is mediated by general fitness and mutation load and is associated with individual differences in levels of 'genetic g'. These two sources (along with a common source of environmental variance) combine to give rise to a variety of cognitive phenotypes characterized by different combinations of high or low levels of 'genetic g' and cognitive specialism or generalism. Fundamental to this model is the assumption that measures of life history speed (K) and g are essentially independent, which is demonstrated via meta-analysis of 10 studies reporting correlations between the variables (ρ = .023, ns, n = 2056). The implications of the model are discussed in an evolutionary, ecological, and developmental context. Seven key predictions are made in the discussion which if tested could provide definitive evidence for the hypothesis. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
78.
针对体育场馆类空旷大跨度结构的设计特点,运用有限元程序,对某公司文化体育中心结构的整体受力性能、薄弱部位及大跨度网架屋盖对主体结构受力性能的影响与协同工作等问题进行了模拟分析.计算表明,此类结构具有扭转不规则性,薄弱部位常出现在空旷结构部位和弱连接部位;空旷框架部分存在相对变形大的现象.由于框架柱缺乏有效约束,其计算长度较大,P-△效应明显;网架屋盖在水平力及地震作用下对主体结构有不可忽视的作用,应考虑网架屋盖与下部框架结构的协同工作.实际设计中可考虑将网架部分按照一定的规则等代为钢梁,来近似考虑网架的刚度进行整体设计.  相似文献   
79.
Does psi exist? D. J. Bem (2011) conducted 9 studies with over 1,000 participants in an attempt to demonstrate that future events retroactively affect people's responses. Here we discuss several limitations of Bem's experiments on psi; in particular, we show that the data analysis was partly exploratory and that one-sided p values may overstate the statistical evidence against the null hypothesis. We reanalyze Bem's data with a default Bayesian t test and show that the evidence for psi is weak to nonexistent. We argue that in order to convince a skeptical audience of a controversial claim, one needs to conduct strictly confirmatory studies and analyze the results with statistical tests that are conservative rather than liberal. We conclude that Bem's p values do not indicate evidence in favor of precognition; instead, they indicate that experimental psychologists need to change the way they conduct their experiments and analyze their data. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
80.
Objective: Past research has investigated age differences in frontal-based decision making, but few studies have focused on the behavioral effects of striatal-based changes in healthy aging. Feedback learning has been found to vary with dopamine levels; increases in dopamine facilitate learning from positive feedback, whereas decreases facilitate learning from negative feedback. Given previous evidence of striatal dopamine depletion in healthy aging, we investigated behavioral differences between college-aged and healthy older adults using a feedback learning task that is sensitive to both frontal and striatal processes. Method: Seventeen college-aged (M = 18.9 years) and 24 healthy, older adults (M = 70.3 years) completed the Probabilistic Selection task, in which participants are trained on probabilistic stimulus-outcome information and then tested to determine whether they learned more from positive or negative feedback. Results: As a group, the older adults learned equally well from positive and negative feedback, whereas the college-aged group learned more from positive than negative feedback, F(1, 39) = 4.10, p effect = .3. However, these group differences were not due to older individuals being more balanced learners. Most individuals of both ages were balanced learners, but while all of the remaining young learners had a positive bias, the remaining older learners were split between those with positive and negative learning biases (χ2(2) = 6.12, p  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号