首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   13104篇
  免费   1592篇
  国内免费   816篇
电工技术   1904篇
技术理论   2篇
综合类   1473篇
化学工业   941篇
金属工艺   321篇
机械仪表   1046篇
建筑科学   660篇
矿业工程   280篇
能源动力   198篇
轻工业   202篇
水利工程   207篇
石油天然气   364篇
武器工业   262篇
无线电   3807篇
一般工业技术   1117篇
冶金工业   588篇
原子能技术   89篇
自动化技术   2051篇
  2024年   66篇
  2023年   255篇
  2022年   326篇
  2021年   440篇
  2020年   462篇
  2019年   369篇
  2018年   358篇
  2017年   465篇
  2016年   509篇
  2015年   555篇
  2014年   844篇
  2013年   817篇
  2012年   1004篇
  2011年   1063篇
  2010年   777篇
  2009年   833篇
  2008年   751篇
  2007年   936篇
  2006年   719篇
  2005年   680篇
  2004年   529篇
  2003年   507篇
  2002年   384篇
  2001年   342篇
  2000年   313篇
  1999年   226篇
  1998年   177篇
  1997年   134篇
  1996年   150篇
  1995年   110篇
  1994年   85篇
  1993年   52篇
  1992年   67篇
  1991年   47篇
  1990年   34篇
  1989年   34篇
  1988年   18篇
  1987年   8篇
  1986年   9篇
  1985年   8篇
  1984年   10篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   5篇
  1966年   2篇
  1965年   3篇
  1964年   3篇
  1961年   2篇
  1959年   2篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 281 毫秒
41.
Two experiments examined how cross-list directional associations influenced list-method directed forgetting and the degree of interference observed on each list. Each List 1 item had a (a) bidirectionally related item on List 2 (chip ←→ potato), (b) forward association with an item on List 2 (chip → wood), (c) backward association from an item on List 2 (chip ← chisel), or (d) no relationship with List 2 items. The results revealed that associative relationships that eliminated retroactive interference in the baseline condition also eliminated the directed forgetting costs. In contrast, associative relationships did not affect List 2 recall in the forget group, which remained unchanged across experimental conditions. However, certain conditions reduced proactive interference in the remember group, thereby eliminating the benefits of directed forgetting. The directed forgetting costs and benefits were observed independently of each other. The authors propose that these effects emerged from a combination of item and context strengthening induced by different associative directions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
42.
For 30 years, A. G. Greenwald and H. G. Shulman's (1973) psychological refractory period (PRP) study has been cited as evidence for perfect timesharing with ideomotor (IM)-compatible tasks. Recently, M.-C. Lien, R. W. Proctor, and P. A. Allen (2002) failed to replicate these results and concluded that IM compatibility is neither necessary nor sufficient to eliminate the PRP effect. A. G. Greenwald (2003) attributed Lien et al.'s nonreplication to the use of (a) a non-IM-compatible task, (b) varied trial spacing, and/or (c) inappropriate instructions. The authors of the present article argue that the first 2 factors are not critical and that instructions merely affect the criterion for speed versus accuracy. In each of Greenwald's experiments, dual-task costs were evident on response time or error rates. Furthermore, the small dual-task costs in his study are consistent with a bottleneck model. Thus, Greenwald (2003) does not provide evidence that IM-compatible tasks enable perfect timesharing. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
43.
It has been reported that it is harder to switch to a strong, well-practiced task from a weaker, less-practiced task than vice versa. Three experiments replicated this surprising asymmetry and investigated how it is affected by a reduction in interference between tasks. Experiment 1 progressively delayed the onset of the stimulus attribute associated with the stronger task. Experiments 2 and 3 separated the response sets of the tasks. Both manipulations reduced, without eliminating, interference of the stronger with the weaker task but reversed the asymmetry of switch costs, resulting in a larger cost of switching to the weaker task. The results are interpreted in terms of a model of the interactions between control input, task strength, and task priming. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
44.
潘静 《冶金动力》2003,(5):20-21,27
从理论上分析了产生地电流干扰的原因,从而在实践上提出了一些经常被忽略,却是行之有效的方法来减小地电流的干扰,设计出高质量的电路。  相似文献   
45.
小型微/纳米级三坐标测量机的研制   总被引:11,自引:1,他引:10  
介绍一套低成本小型三维坐标测量机的研制过程,作为微/纳米级超精密测量设备,整体机台结构采用花岗岩材料;X/Y轴的移动平台利用压电材料摩擦驱动与挠性变形原理,长短两行程可达25mm范围及5nm的微位移,藉自行研发的衍射干涉激光尺,位置回馈测量系统可具有0.025nm的分辨率;测量探头在一般DVD光读取头的自动聚焦原理基础上改装,最佳分辨率可达2.5nm,该雏形机适于测量微机电组件、微型器件及膜厚等。  相似文献   
46.
An increasing demand for high data rate transmission and protection over bandlimited channels with severe inter-symbol interference has resulted in a flurry of activity to improve channel equalization. In conjunction with equalization, channel coding-decoding can be employed to improve system performance. In this letter, the performance of the three stageturbo-equalization-decoding employing log maximum a posteriori probability is experimentally evaluated by a fading simulator. The BER is evaluated using various information sequence and interleaver sizes taking into account that the communication medium is a noisy inter symbol interference channel.  相似文献   
47.
量子阱红外探测器能带结构的计算   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
利用电子波在阱与垒的界面上的反射及干涉效应,计算了量子阱红外探测器(QWIP)的能带结构,并对其适用性进行了分析和讨论。通过与K-P模型比较发现,本方法对计算较宽势阱(阱宽大于4nm)的量子阱结构的电子态适合。在垒宽和阱宽不变条件下,用两种方法计算得到的AlGaAs/GaAs量子阱材料中Al组分x与吸收峰值波长λp的关系曲线基本相同。结果说明,在较宽的范围内,本方法对QWIP能带结构的计算是适用且简便的。  相似文献   
48.
This paper reports how the study of formal logical reasoning provides insight into more everyday types of reasoning, such as that involved in language comprehension. Both of these types of cognition are thought to involve the use of mental models, and so it is reasonable to think that the cognitive operations needed for formal logical reasoning would be involved in everyday reasoning as well. We focused on three aspects of formal reasoning: (a) the integration of information into a common mental model, (b) the drawing of inferences, and (c) the coordination of alternative possibilities. We were able to show that the integration and inference components were related to narrative comprehension processes, but the coordination of alternative models was not. Thus, there is evidence for some overlap in the mental processes used in formal and everyday reasoning. This further justifies the study of formal logical reasoning as a window into certain types of everyday reasoning. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
49.
Through two experiments, the study sought to emphasize the usefulness of the visual and kinesthetic imagery in mental practice. In Experiment 1, it was hypothesized that when the task to be learned through mental practice necessitates the reproduction of a form by drawing, the visual image, which provides a wide span of apprehension, is more suitable than the kinesthetic image. On the other hand, the kinesthetic image that supplies inputs from the muscles' positions and movements should be more appropriate for the acquisition of the duration of the drawing. In Experiment 2, it was hypothesized that the task, transformed into a motor task necessitating minute coordination of the two hands, would benefit more from kinesthetic imagery. To have optimal control over what was actually experienced during mental practice, the participants' imagery skills were measured. The participants also benefited from prior imagery training. The results demonstrate that when using mental practice to initially acquire a task, visual imagery is better for tasks that emphasize form while kinesthetic imagery is better for those tasks that emphasize timing or minute coordination of the two hands. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
50.
多模干涉型集成光学器件研究进展   总被引:4,自引:1,他引:3  
基于自镜像效应(SIE)的多模干涉(MMI)型集成光学器件是集成光学中具有广泛用途的重要器件。本文详细而系统地综述了近几年来MMI器在国内外的研究进展,特别是其在集成光学中的应用。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号