首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1765篇
  免费   177篇
  国内免费   69篇
电工技术   134篇
综合类   128篇
化学工业   86篇
金属工艺   32篇
机械仪表   37篇
建筑科学   188篇
矿业工程   16篇
能源动力   71篇
轻工业   102篇
水利工程   97篇
石油天然气   17篇
武器工业   6篇
无线电   127篇
一般工业技术   218篇
冶金工业   440篇
原子能技术   13篇
自动化技术   299篇
  2024年   7篇
  2023年   27篇
  2022年   43篇
  2021年   52篇
  2020年   52篇
  2019年   57篇
  2018年   60篇
  2017年   74篇
  2016年   77篇
  2015年   67篇
  2014年   95篇
  2013年   101篇
  2012年   99篇
  2011年   136篇
  2010年   91篇
  2009年   98篇
  2008年   87篇
  2007年   112篇
  2006年   106篇
  2005年   90篇
  2004年   78篇
  2003年   99篇
  2002年   75篇
  2001年   54篇
  2000年   19篇
  1999年   30篇
  1998年   14篇
  1997年   14篇
  1996年   12篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   14篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   3篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   2篇
  1978年   2篇
  1965年   2篇
  1964年   2篇
  1963年   5篇
  1961年   8篇
  1959年   1篇
  1958年   1篇
  1957年   1篇
  1956年   3篇
  1955年   3篇
排序方式: 共有2011条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
Despite controversial expectations that animals achieve reciprocal altruism, it is unclear if nonhuman species possess the necessary cognitive abilities. For reciprocal altruism, individuals must anticipate the loss of a commodity and accept a delay before some return. The authors investigated the abilities of 5 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) to cope with increasing waiting duration in exchange tasks. Subjects had to keep a small cookie before returning it to a human partner to obtain a larger piece. For a piece 2, 4, or 8 times the size of the small piece, 3 of the 5 subjects waited for up to 4 min. For a piece 40 times larger, 4 of the 5 subjects waited up to 8 min. At long time lag, renouncement to wait occurred earlier than predicted by subjects' general waiting capacity, suggesting that the decision to wait was based on a trade-off between reward quantity and expected costs of the waiting duration. Chimpanzees could anticipate a delayed reward at a time scale of several minutes. If this reflects a cognitive limit in chimpanzees' anticipation capacity, reciprocal altruism by keeping track of costs and benefits over extended periods may be unlikely in chimpanzees. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
992.
The authors examined the effect of time perception and sense of progress in telephone queues on caller reactions to 3 telephone waiting time fillers: music, apologies, and information about location in the queue. In Study 1, conducted on 123 real calls, call abandonment was lowest, and call evaluations were most positive with information about location in the queue as the time filler. In Study 2, conducted with 83 participants who experienced a simulated telephone wait experience, sense of progress in the queue rather than perceived waiting time mediated the relationship between telephone waiting time filler and caller reactions. The findings provide insight for the management and design of telephone queues, as well as theoretical insight into critical cognitive processes that underlie telephone waiting, opening up an important new research agenda. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
993.
在电容器未经研究充电和预充电两种超始条件下,对绝缘电阻的测量值进行瞬态分析,定量地导出两种起始条件下测量值与仪器内阻、电容器固有特性之间的关系。为分析测量结果和制定标准提供依据。  相似文献   
994.
The number, amplitude, duration, and bout structure of isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalization (USV) of infant rats (Rattus norvegicus) were measured on postnatal Day 10. Measurements were made before and after a brief, 1-min, active interaction with their mother or before and after a "pick-up" control procedure. Consistent with prior studies, the number of USVs emitted was significantly increased in the period following the maternal reunion but not after the control procedure. The average amplitude of USVs was also greater following maternal reunion. Finally, analyses characterizing the bout structure of USV production indicated that the average bout size (i.e.. number of USVs/bout) was increased severalfold following the reunion with the mother, accounting for the greater rate of USV production during the second isolation period. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
995.
On many cyclic-interval schedules, animals adjust their postreinforcement pause to follow the interval duration (temporal tracking). Six pigeons were trained on a series of square-wave (2-valued) interval schedules (e.g., 12 fixed-interval [FI] 60, 4 FI 180). Experiment 1 showed that pigeons track square-wave schedules, except those with a single long interval per cycle. Experiments 2 and 3 established that tracking and nontracking are learned and both can transfer from one cyclic schedule to another. Experiment 4 demonstrated that pigeons track a schedule with a single short interval per cycle, suggesting that a dual process--cuing and tracking--is necessary to explain behavior on these schedules. These findings suggest a potential explanation for earlier results that reported a failure to track square-wave schedules. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
996.
冯世新  叶清林 《南方金属》2003,(4):49-50,57
利用数学方法对不均匀沉降引起的建筑物或基坑等变形数据进行分析,估算建筑物沉降的稳定时间和最大沉降值,能够及时对构筑物进行纠偏.  相似文献   
997.
现代钢结构抗火设计方法   总被引:14,自引:3,他引:11  
文章论述了火灾对负结构的危害和钢结构抗火设计的目标与意义,基于高温承载力极限状态验算的思想,文章阐述了现代钢结构抗火计算与设计方法,并通过试验验证了这一方法的科学性、合理性与有效性。  相似文献   
998.
Formulas are derived for probabilities of stationary states of a queuing system with demands of different types and a discipline of the inverse type with interrupts. Later on, the service of an interrupted demand is completed or such a demand is served again with a new (or with an old) length depending on its type.  相似文献   
999.
Volatile compounds from tomatoes were measured in the headspace above tomatoes and in the air expired from the noses of people eating tomatoes (nosespace). Eleven target compounds, representative of the different metabolic pathways that contribute to tomato aroma were chosen for analysis. The procedure consisted of trapping volumes of headspace or nosespace on Tenax, desorbing and chromatographing the samples on gas chromatographs and quantification by integration of characteristic ion chromatograms. Small volumes (8.5 cm3) of headspace were used to develop the method which was then applied to study potential sources of variation in the raw material and in the sampling procedure. The variation in headspace profiles, with time after dicing tomatoes, and with tissue maturity, demonstrated that the amounts of some volatiles changed with time and with maturity. Sampling was therefore undertaken using batches of tomatoes with similar histories and the time of sampling was fixed. The headspace and nosespace profiles from tomatoes produced raw data with substantial variation (percentage coefficient of variation 50–60%) but this appeared to be due to different amounts of voiatiles in the replicates. When data were expressed on a relative basis by normalisation, the profiles from groups of replicates were seen to be quite similar for nine of the compounds but values for 3-methylnitrobutane and 2-isobutylthiazole showed considerable variation. Nosespace profiles of tomato voiatiles were broadly similar between operators when expressed on a relative basis; the actual total amounts varied considerably. The headspace profiles from diced and stomached tomatoes were comparable but distinctly different from the nosespace profile. There were differences between the headspace and nosespace profiles particularly in the amounts of 3-methylbutanal, dimethyl disulphide and hexanal which were present to a greater extent in the headspace on a relative basis.  相似文献   
1000.
Presents a reply by the authors to comments by Sullivan (see record 2007-14606-016) and Webster (see record 2007-14606-017). Both of these comments help to place our article on the red states versus blue states metaphor (Seyle & Newman, see record 2006-11202-001) into a broader context. In particular, both comments make valuable points about the potential for collaboration between psychology, political science, and political action. Sullivan made two points that address this collaboration directly. First, he pointed out that we failed to acknowledge a book by Morris Fiorina and his colleagues. Second, Sullivan disagreed with our claim that psychologists cannot change the system. Webster argued persuasively that we let the Electoral College off too easy. To support his indictment of the Electoral College, he drew on two compelling sources of evidence. First, because of changes in population distribution, electoral votes appear to play more of a role in the outcome of elections than they have at any time in the past. Second, the weighting system used to determine a state's number of electoral votes has the consequence of underrepresenting minority populations. However, we are less optimistic than Webster that this approach might "eliminate red- blue labeling" (p. 703). Thus, we repeat our original suggestion that the best short-term solution for improving political discourse is to encourage the use of the "purple America" approach. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号