首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   14867篇
  免费   1957篇
  国内免费   1544篇
电工技术   713篇
技术理论   4篇
综合类   1914篇
化学工业   388篇
金属工艺   297篇
机械仪表   1036篇
建筑科学   798篇
矿业工程   171篇
能源动力   180篇
轻工业   287篇
水利工程   242篇
石油天然气   342篇
武器工业   203篇
无线电   1296篇
一般工业技术   1544篇
冶金工业   682篇
原子能技术   109篇
自动化技术   8162篇
  2024年   116篇
  2023年   340篇
  2022年   518篇
  2021年   540篇
  2020年   496篇
  2019年   444篇
  2018年   369篇
  2017年   420篇
  2016年   552篇
  2015年   548篇
  2014年   760篇
  2013年   973篇
  2012年   878篇
  2011年   1025篇
  2010年   871篇
  2009年   955篇
  2008年   977篇
  2007年   1125篇
  2006年   995篇
  2005年   819篇
  2004年   739篇
  2003年   692篇
  2002年   538篇
  2001年   461篇
  2000年   406篇
  1999年   328篇
  1998年   228篇
  1997年   161篇
  1996年   163篇
  1995年   178篇
  1994年   129篇
  1993年   118篇
  1992年   95篇
  1991年   85篇
  1990年   64篇
  1989年   67篇
  1988年   53篇
  1987年   22篇
  1986年   24篇
  1985年   17篇
  1984年   8篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   10篇
  1979年   7篇
  1963年   4篇
  1960年   3篇
  1959年   3篇
  1958年   3篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
常速度梯度射线追踪与二维层速度反演   总被引:6,自引:1,他引:5  
速度问题是勘探地震学的核心 ,可以借助常速度梯度射线追踪方法 ,从反演的角度探讨速度建模问题。在射线追踪方面 ,从程函方程出发 ,将复杂的速度场剖分成小的矩形单元 ,并假设在每个矩形单元内速度具有常速度梯度 ,由此导出的一组常速度梯度射线追踪公式 ,可实现非均匀介质的射线追踪 ,具有适应性强、精度高和快速的优点。在层速度反演方面 ,采用相干速度反演的思想 ,根据各向同性介质中 ,零偏移距射线在界面处是法向入射的原理 ,建立了合成共中心点道集与射线深度偏移的正反演关系 ,由此利用合成共中心点道集与观测的共中心点道集逼近 ,采用层剥离方法进行层速度反演。通过理论推导与模型试算得到如下的认识与结论 :常速度梯度射线追踪具有较高的计算精度与效率 ,公式采用向量形式很容易推广到三维 ;确定层速度采用的是正反演相结合的方法 ,可得到稳定的速度场 ;该方法屏弃了基于双曲时矩关系确定层速度场的假设 ,使其能适应任意的速度分布。  相似文献   
102.
In the case study presented in this paper we consider early development phases of a mechanical product. We want to evaluate different concepts and decide which one(s) to pursue. A problem in early phases is that usually no test runs are available. In our case study, based on a standard, there are ways to compute the lifetime distributions of the components of the different concepts. Some parameters needed for these computations are not known precisely. Unfortunately, the lifetime distributions of the components are highly sensitive to these parameters. Our approach is to equip these parameters with distributions. These distributions would be called prior distributions in Bayesian terminology, but no update is possible since no test runs are available. Our approach implies that the distribution of the system lifetime for each concept is random, i.e. we get random elements in the space of lifetime distributions. Using Monte-Carlo simulations, we demonstrate several ways to compare the random lifetime distributions of the concepts. Some of these comparisons use stochastic orderings. We also introduce a new stochastic ordering which is particularly suitable for reliability purposes. Our case study, consisting of three scenarios, allows us to demonstrate some conclusions that can be reached.  相似文献   
103.
The aim of this study was to bring to the surface the strategic use of imitative processes in the context of a 2-route model: (a) direct imitation, used in reproducing new, meaningless actions, and (b) imitation based on stored semantic knowledge of familiar meaningful actions. Three experiments were carried out with healthy participants who reproduced meaningful and meaningless actions within an established time limit. The study investigated 3 factors that could potentially affect the selection of processes used for imitation: (a) the composition of the experimental list (blocked or mixed presentation), (b) the presence-absence of instructions (Experiments 1 and 2), and (c) the relative proportions of the stimuli (Experiment 3). Overall, the results suggest that each of these factors influences the selection of imitative strategies in healthy individuals with temporarily reduced capacities, as happens in the case of brain-damaged patients. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
104.
知识经济对领导方法提出了新的要求,也使传统的领导方法面临严峻挑战,知识经济时代所需要的领导方法具有开拓创新性。  相似文献   
105.
People have knowledge about relationships (i.e., relational schemas) that is based on their experiences. Because most people have experience with complementary behavior (interaction partners behaving similarly in terms of affiliation but oppositely in terms of control), they expect complementary behavior in their relationships. Like other beliefs about relationships, expectations of complementarity affect self-construal. The authors provide evidence for complementary self-construal; people assimilate to relevant relationship partners on the affiliation dimension and contrast on the control dimension. Consistent with the proposed role of relationship knowledge in these effects, complementary self-construal was moderated by the familiarity of the target, whether people focused on their relationship with or the appearance of the target, and whether the context was relevant for the interpersonal dimension. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
106.
知识经济的发展,使知识和创新,成为人类活动的强大动力,人类的实践活动迈入了崭新阶段。人类的潜在智慧和才能,正源源不断地释放出来,转化为对象性存在,充分展示了人类活动的主体性效应,与此同时我们也必须正视人类活动的反主体性的效应。正确处理主体自觉能动性和客体自然性之间矛盾,是强化人类活动的主体性效应,防止反主体性效应的前提条件。  相似文献   
107.
虚拟企业中的知识扩散与技术创新   总被引:13,自引:1,他引:12  
采用知识链方法对虚拟企业的技术和知识扩散与创新的方式和途径进行了初步探索,将虚拟企业中知识扩散方式分为传播扩散型、培养指导型和互动创造型三类模式;同时,认为信任机制是实现虚拟企业中成员企业知识特别是隐形知识的交流与共享的前提,是实现技术和知识创新的基础。  相似文献   
108.
Causal learning requires integrating constraints provided by domain-specific theories with domain-general statistical learning. In order to investigate the interaction between these factors, the authors presented preschoolers with stories pitting their existing theories against statistical evidence. Each child heard 2 stories in which 2 candidate causes co-occurred with an effect. Evidence was presented in the form: AB→E; CA→E; AD→E; and so forth. In 1 story, all variables came from the same domain; in the other, the recurring candidate cause, A, came from a different domain (A was a psychological cause of a biological effect). After receiving this statistical evidence, children were asked to identify the cause of the effect on a new trial. Consistent with the predictions of a Bayesian model, all children were more likely to identify A as the cause within domains than across domains. Whereas 3.5-year-olds learned only from the within-domain evidence, 4- and 5-year-olds learned from the cross-domain evidence and were able to transfer their new expectations about psychosomatic causality to a novel task. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
109.
We propose subspace distance measures to analyze the similarity between intrapersonal face subspaces, which characterize the variations between face images of the same individual. We call the conventional intrapersonal subspace average intrapersonal subspace (AIS) because the image differences often come from a large number of persons. An intrapersonal subspace is referred to as specific intrapersonal subspace (SIS) if the image differences are from just one person. We demonstrate that SIS varies significantly from person to person, and most SISs are not similar to AIS. Based on these observations, we introduce the maximum a posteriori (MAP) adaptation to the problem of SIS estimation, and apply it to the Bayesian face recognition algorithm. Experimental results show that the adaptive Bayesian algorithm outperforms the non-adaptive Bayesian algorithm as well as Eigenface and Fisherface methods if a small number of adaptation images are available.  相似文献   
110.
面向对象机器翻译知识库IMT—KB的设计与实现   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
机器翻译知识库是机译系统的重要组成部分,针对传统机译知识库的不足之处,本文提出一种面向对象的机译知识体系结构,同时给出这种具有层次性和模块性机译知识库的存储组织和管理机制。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号