全文获取类型
收费全文 | 685篇 |
免费 | 47篇 |
国内免费 | 48篇 |
专业分类
电工技术 | 4篇 |
综合类 | 153篇 |
化学工业 | 5篇 |
金属工艺 | 3篇 |
机械仪表 | 10篇 |
建筑科学 | 25篇 |
矿业工程 | 5篇 |
轻工业 | 5篇 |
水利工程 | 3篇 |
石油天然气 | 3篇 |
武器工业 | 1篇 |
无线电 | 15篇 |
一般工业技术 | 15篇 |
冶金工业 | 247篇 |
自动化技术 | 286篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 11篇 |
2022年 | 32篇 |
2021年 | 30篇 |
2020年 | 16篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 16篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 10篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 20篇 |
2013年 | 15篇 |
2012年 | 24篇 |
2011年 | 47篇 |
2010年 | 56篇 |
2009年 | 53篇 |
2008年 | 42篇 |
2007年 | 50篇 |
2006年 | 51篇 |
2005年 | 45篇 |
2004年 | 53篇 |
2003年 | 33篇 |
2002年 | 32篇 |
2001年 | 27篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1965年 | 2篇 |
1964年 | 2篇 |
1961年 | 1篇 |
1957年 | 2篇 |
1956年 | 3篇 |
1955年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有780条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
The evolution and maintenance of large-scale software systems requires first an understanding of its architecture before delving
into lower-level details. Tools facilitating the architecture comprehension tasks by visualization provide different sets
of configurable, graphical elements to present information to their users. We conducted a controlled experiment that exemplifies
the critical role of such graphical elements when aiming at understanding the architecture. In our setting, a different configuration
of graphical elements had significant influence on program comprehension tasks. In particular, a 63% gain in effectiveness
in architectural analysis tasks was achieved simply by changing the configuration of the graphical elements of the same tool.
Based on the results, we claim that significant effort should be spent on the configuration of architecture visualization
tools and that configurability should be a requirement for such tools.
Jens Knodel is a scientist at the Fraunhofer Institute for Experimental Software Engineering (IESE) in Kaiserslautern, Germany. As an applied researcher in the department “Product Line Architectures” he works in several industrial and research projects in the context of product line engineering and software architectures. His main research interests are architecture compliance checking, software evolution, and architecture reconstruction. Jens Knodel is the architect of the Fraunhofer SAVE tool (the acronym SAVE stands for Software Architecture Evaluation and Visualization). Dirk Muthig heads the division “Software Development” at the Fraunhofer Institute for Experimental Software Engineering (IESE). He has been involved in the definition, development, and transfer of Fraunhofer PuLSE (Product Line Software Engineering) methodology since 1997. Further, he leads the research and technology transfer in the area of “Software and Systems Architecture”. He received a diploma in computer science, as well as a Ph.D., from the Technical University of Kaiserslautern. Matthias Naab is an engineer at the Fraunhofer Institute for Experimental Software Engineering (IESE). He works in the areas of software- and system architectures and product lines. In several industry projects, he was involved in architecture evaluations of large-scale information systems from different industries and customers. To the Fraunhofer SAVE tool, he contributed the visualization component. Matthias Naab received a diploma in computer science from the Technical University of Kaiserslautern in 2005. 相似文献
Matthias Naab (Corresponding author)Email: |
Jens Knodel is a scientist at the Fraunhofer Institute for Experimental Software Engineering (IESE) in Kaiserslautern, Germany. As an applied researcher in the department “Product Line Architectures” he works in several industrial and research projects in the context of product line engineering and software architectures. His main research interests are architecture compliance checking, software evolution, and architecture reconstruction. Jens Knodel is the architect of the Fraunhofer SAVE tool (the acronym SAVE stands for Software Architecture Evaluation and Visualization). Dirk Muthig heads the division “Software Development” at the Fraunhofer Institute for Experimental Software Engineering (IESE). He has been involved in the definition, development, and transfer of Fraunhofer PuLSE (Product Line Software Engineering) methodology since 1997. Further, he leads the research and technology transfer in the area of “Software and Systems Architecture”. He received a diploma in computer science, as well as a Ph.D., from the Technical University of Kaiserslautern. Matthias Naab is an engineer at the Fraunhofer Institute for Experimental Software Engineering (IESE). He works in the areas of software- and system architectures and product lines. In several industry projects, he was involved in architecture evaluations of large-scale information systems from different industries and customers. To the Fraunhofer SAVE tool, he contributed the visualization component. Matthias Naab received a diploma in computer science from the Technical University of Kaiserslautern in 2005. 相似文献
42.
43.
44.
中文电子病历实体包含大量的医学领域词汇并具有明显的嵌套特征。嵌套实体识别时往往存在目标实体定位不完整、不准确的问题。针对这一问题,提出了一种基于机器阅读理解的中文电子病历嵌套命名实体识别模型MRC-PBM (machine reading comprehension-position information biaffine and MLP)。该模型将命名实体识别(named entity recognition, NER)转化为机器阅读理解任务,将中文电子病历文本和预定义的查询语句串联作为输入,使用基于医学的预训练模型MC_BERT获取词向量,然后通过双向长短期记忆网络模型(BiLSTM)和多粒度扩张卷积模型分别获取双向的特征信息以及单词之间的信息,得到相应的特征向量,最后使用Hybrid-PBM预测器进行实体预测。在嵌套和平面NER数据集上进行实验。实验表明,该模型在糖尿病语料和公开医学数据集上优于其他主流神经网络模型,F1值比基线模型提高了1.21%~5.80%。 相似文献
45.
Laypersons, the media, and many legal scholars tend to attribute problems in the jury system to the dispositions of individual jurors and to recommend reforms in jury selection procedures and relaxation of the unanimity rule. Social scientists view problems as a consequence of the structure of the jurors' task and recommend reforms in trial procedures. After years of apathy, the legal system has proposed, and in some jurisdictions implemented, a variety of reforms, most of which are based on the social science perspective that the problem is not due to bad jurors but to unnecessary procedural obstacles to high-quality decision making. These reforms are described in the final section of the article. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
46.
The identification of abstractions within existing software systems is an important problem to be solved to facilitate program comprehension and the construction of a set of reusable artifacts. In particular, of interest is the identification of object-like features in procedural programs. Existing techniques and algorithms achieve some level of success but do not, in general, always precisely identify a coherent set of objects. The identified objects tend to contain spurious methods that are only tenuously related to the object and require a great deal of human effort and understanding to unravel. This paper presents an improved algorithm that overcomes these drawbacks and enables the precise identification of objects with less human intervention and understanding by exploiting simple statistical techniques. The algorithm is applied to several sample programs and the results are compared with existing algorithms. Finally, the application of the algorithm to a real medium-size system is described and discussed. The algorithm was developed as part of the RE2 project in which the identification of object-like features in existing systems is the basis for a re-engineering process aimed at populating a repository of reusable assets. 相似文献
47.
Williams Joanna P.; Stafford K. Brooke; Lauer Kristen D.; Hall Kendra M.; Pollini Simonne 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2009,101(1):1
This study evaluated the effectiveness of comprehension training embedded in a program that taught science content to 2nd graders. The program included instruction about the structure of compare-contrast expository text, emphasizing clue words, generic questions, graphic organizers, and the close analysis of well-structured text exemplars. This program was compared with a program that focused on the science content but included no compare-contrast training as well as with a no-instruction control. Regular classroom teachers (14 from 4 schools), randomly assigned to treatment, provided the instruction; 215 students (7-8 years old) participated. The study replicated acquisition and transfer effects found in an earlier study, that is, transfer to compare-contrast text with content related and unrelated to the instructional content (with no loss in the amount of science content acquired). The program also led to better performance on written and oral response measures and on 1 of the 2 measures involving authentic (less well-structured) compare-contrast text. These findings support and extend previous findings that explicit instruction in comprehension is effective as early as the primary-grade level. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
48.
Is Skim reading effective? How do readers allocate their attention selectively? The authors report 3 experiments that use expository texts and allow readers only enough time to read half of each document. Experiment 1 found that, relative to reading half the text, skimming improved memory for important ideas from a text but did not improve memory of less important details or of inferences made from information within the text. Experiment 2 found no advantage of skimming over reading the first or second half of every paragraph. Two final experiments using a hierarchical, Website-like layout of documents showed that the advantage of skimming found in Experiment 1 was dependent on the linkages between pages and, thus, the ease with which participants could navigate through the text. Data on page-by-page reading times and eye-tracking analyses from Experiment 2 indicated that Skim readers spent more time reading text that was earlier in the paragraph, toward the top of the page and in an earlier page of the document. These findings were interpreted as evidence in support of a “satisficing” account of skimming process. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
49.
Reports an error in "The contributions of working memory and executive functioning to problem representation and solution generation in algebraic word problems" by Kerry Lee, Ee Lynn Ng and Swee Fong Ng (Journal of Educational Psychology, 2009[May], Vol 101[2], 373-387). In this article, the URL published for the supplemental material was incorrect. The correct URL is http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013843.supp. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2009-04640-010.) Solving algebraic word problems involves multiple cognitive phases. The authors used a multitask approach to examine the extent to which working memory and executive functioning are associated with generating problem models and producing solutions. They tested 255 11-year-olds on working memory (Counting Recall, Letter Memory, and Keep Track), ability to inhibit inappropriate responses (inhibition: numeric Stroop, Stop Signal), mental flexibility (switching: Number–Letter and Plus–Minus), English literacy, and algebraic problem-solving skills (problem representation, solution generation, and other subcomponents). Working memory explained about a quarter of the variance in both representation and solution formation. Literacy explained an additional 20% of the variance in representation formation. Ability to discern quantitative relationships explained an additional 10%. The findings go beyond a demonstration of an association between working memory and problem-solving accuracy. They show that success in word problems is particularly reliant on ability to decode and assign mathematical operators to quantitative relationships, 2 phases of problem solving that also draw heavily on working memory resources. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
50.