首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   40896篇
  免费   5201篇
  国内免费   3280篇
电工技术   4083篇
技术理论   1篇
综合类   4461篇
化学工业   3794篇
金属工艺   1039篇
机械仪表   2068篇
建筑科学   2876篇
矿业工程   912篇
能源动力   1093篇
轻工业   2049篇
水利工程   844篇
石油天然气   1621篇
武器工业   630篇
无线电   5486篇
一般工业技术   3817篇
冶金工业   1916篇
原子能技术   576篇
自动化技术   12111篇
  2024年   207篇
  2023年   553篇
  2022年   1039篇
  2021年   1185篇
  2020年   1483篇
  2019年   1399篇
  2018年   1238篇
  2017年   1622篇
  2016年   1775篇
  2015年   1881篇
  2014年   2528篇
  2013年   2916篇
  2012年   2887篇
  2011年   3042篇
  2010年   2294篇
  2009年   2438篇
  2008年   2325篇
  2007年   2731篇
  2006年   2566篇
  2005年   2111篇
  2004年   1719篇
  2003年   1610篇
  2002年   1391篇
  2001年   1187篇
  2000年   1015篇
  1999年   754篇
  1998年   606篇
  1997年   537篇
  1996年   403篇
  1995年   333篇
  1994年   279篇
  1993年   228篇
  1992年   164篇
  1991年   153篇
  1990年   109篇
  1989年   94篇
  1988年   67篇
  1987年   68篇
  1986年   53篇
  1985年   55篇
  1984年   41篇
  1983年   31篇
  1982年   32篇
  1981年   28篇
  1980年   24篇
  1979年   22篇
  1978年   13篇
  1977年   12篇
  1964年   12篇
  1956年   11篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 1 毫秒
101.
分析了重油催化裂化沉降器结焦的原因,认为反应油气中含有催化剂颗粒以及油气中重组分的冷凝是沉降器结焦的物理原因,而重芳烃、胶质、沥青质的高温缩合则是沉降器结焦的化学因素。因此,防止沉降器结焦的关键是抑制油气重组分的冷凝和减少油气在沉降器内的停留时间。  相似文献   
102.
Abstract. This article proposes an extension to scalar component methodology for the identification and estimation of VARMA models. The complete methodology determines the exact positions of all free parameters in any VARMA model with a predetermined embedded scalar component structure. This leads to an exactly identified system of equations that is estimated using full information maximum likelihood.  相似文献   
103.
一种PN码自适应捕获门限的改进算法   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
刘震昆  黄顺吉 《信号处理》2006,22(4):458-461
文献[1]提出了一种用于直扩系统的PN码自适应门限算法。但该文献也指出,此算法对门限总数十分敏感。当门限总数设置不当时,系统的平均捕获时间将显著增加。这限制了该算法在实际中的应用。本文就此提出了改进,给出了算法及电路框图。仿真结果表明,与原方案相比,改进算法改善了对门限总数的敏感性,降低了PN码平均捕获时间。  相似文献   
104.
Broadcasting is the process of spreading one piece of information among a group of individuals connected by an interconnection network. In this paper we give exact lower and upper bounds for the number of broadcast schemes in arbitrary networks. Also, we give the exact value for complete bipartite graphs and an upper bound for regular networks. Based on the counting method we describe a new random algorithm for broadcasting in networks.  相似文献   
105.
Learning and memory of novel spatial configurations aids behaviors such as visual search through an implicit process called contextual cuing (M. M. Chun & Y. Jiang, 1998). The present study provides rigorous tests of the implicit nature of contextual cuing. Experiment 1 used a recognition test that closely matched the learning task, confirming that memory traces of predictive spatial context were not accessible to conscious retrieval. Experiment 2 gave explicit instructions to encode visual context during learning, but learning was not improved and conscious memory remained undetectable. Experiment 3 illustrates that memory traces for spatial context may persist for at least 1 week, suggesting a long-term component of contextual cuing. These experiments indicate that the learning and memory of spatial context in the contextual cuing task are indeed implicit. The results have implications for understanding the neural substrate of spatial contextual learning, which may depend on an intact medial temporal lobe system that includes the hippocampus (M. M. Chun & E. A. Phelps, 1999). (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
106.
Phoneme identification with audiovisually discrepant stimuli is influenced hy information in the visual signal (the McGurk effect). Additionally, lexical status affects identification of auditorily presented phonemes. The present study tested for lexical influences on the McGurk effect. Participants identified phonemes in audiovisually discrepant stimuli in which lexical status of the auditory component and of a visually influenced percept was independently varied. Visually influenced (McGurk) responses were more frequent when they formed a word and when the auditory signal was a nonword (Experiment 1). Lexical effects were larger for slow than for fast responses (Experiment 2), as with auditory speech, and were replicated with stimuli matched on physical properties (Experiment 3). These results are consistent with models in which lexical processing of speech is modality independent. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
107.
Lateralized readiness potentials (LRPs) were used to determine the stage(s) of reaction time (RT) responsible for speed-accuracy trade-offs (SATs). Speeded decisions based on several types of information were examined in 3 experiments, involving, respectively, a line discrimination task, lexical decisions, and an Erikson flanker task. Three levels of SAT were obtained in each experiment by adjusting response deadlines with an adaptive tracking algorithm. Speed stress affected the duration of RT stages both before and after the start of the LRP in all experiments. The latter effect cannot be explained by guessing strategies, by variations in response force, or as an indirect consequence of the pre-LRP effect. Contrary to most models, it suggests that SAT can occur at a late postdecisional stage. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
108.
These studies investigated self-regulation and subjective experience of time from the perspective of the regulatory resource model. Studies 1-2 showed that participants who were instructed to regulate their emotions while viewing a film clip perceived that the film lasted longer than participants who did not regulate their emotions. In Study 3, participants provided time estimates during a resource-depleting or nondepleting task. Subsequent task persistence was measured. Time perceptions mediated the effect of initial self-regulation on subsequent self-regulated performance. In Study 4, participants performed either a resource-depleting or a nondepleting thought-listing task and then performed a different regulatory task. Compared with nondepleted participants, depleted participants persisted less on the 2nd task but estimated that they had persisted longer. Subjective time estimates statistically accounted for reduced persistence after depletion. Together, results indicate people believe that self-regulatory endeavors last overly long, a belief that may result in abandonment of further self-control. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
109.
Two experiments examined the disruption of feature-based selection in triple-conjunction search at multiple target transfers. In Experiment 1, after 10 training sessions, a new target possessing previous distractor features was introduced. This produced disruption in RT and fixation number, but no disruption in feature-based selection. Specifically, there was a tendency to fixate objects sharing the target's contrast polarity and shape and this did not change even upon transfer to the new target. In Experiment 2, 30 training sessions were provided with three target transfers. At the first transfer, the results replicated Experiment 1. Subsequent transfers did not produce disruption on any measure. These findings are discussed in terms of strength theory, Guided Search, rule-based approaches to perceptual learning, and the area activation model. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
110.
史秋贤  冯斌  王朝安  吴永红 《测井技术》2003,27(Z1):23-24,32
针对桩1区块特高含水期底水稠油油藏的开发特点,利用核磁共振测井资料定量评价其孔隙流体流动特性,准确划分产层,直接识别油、水层,准确地评价了区块剩余油分布情况.依此部署了水平井,取得了很好的开发效果.实践证明,核磁共振测井具备常规测井所不具备的某些技术优势,能解决一些特殊的地质问题,有广泛的应用前景.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号