首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   914篇
  免费   22篇
电工技术   7篇
化学工业   110篇
金属工艺   22篇
机械仪表   13篇
建筑科学   29篇
矿业工程   1篇
能源动力   6篇
轻工业   81篇
水利工程   12篇
石油天然气   3篇
无线电   128篇
一般工业技术   107篇
冶金工业   340篇
原子能技术   5篇
自动化技术   72篇
  2022年   6篇
  2021年   9篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   8篇
  2018年   7篇
  2017年   6篇
  2016年   9篇
  2015年   12篇
  2014年   12篇
  2013年   39篇
  2012年   20篇
  2011年   35篇
  2010年   23篇
  2009年   18篇
  2008年   24篇
  2007年   27篇
  2006年   19篇
  2005年   23篇
  2004年   31篇
  2003年   16篇
  2002年   24篇
  2001年   17篇
  2000年   14篇
  1999年   29篇
  1998年   78篇
  1997年   43篇
  1996年   41篇
  1995年   20篇
  1994年   19篇
  1993年   27篇
  1992年   11篇
  1991年   10篇
  1990年   14篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   15篇
  1987年   14篇
  1986年   10篇
  1985年   16篇
  1984年   12篇
  1983年   8篇
  1982年   7篇
  1981年   8篇
  1979年   9篇
  1978年   10篇
  1977年   18篇
  1976年   15篇
  1975年   11篇
  1974年   6篇
  1972年   9篇
  1966年   5篇
排序方式: 共有936条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
4.
Biases in social comparative judgments, such as those illustrated by above-average and comparative-optimism effects, are often regarded as products of motivated reasoning (e.g., self-enhancement). These effects, however, can also be produced by information-processing limitations or aspects of judgment processes that are not necessarily biased by motivational factors. In this article, the authors briefly review motivational accounts of biased comparative judgments, introduce a 3-stage model for understanding how people make comparative judgments, and then describe how various nonmotivational factors can influence the 3 stages of the comparative judgment process. Finally, the authors discuss several unresolved issues highlighted by their analysis, such as the interrelation between motivated and nonmotivated sources of bias and the influence of nonmotivated sources of bias on behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
5.
We have developed and implemented techniques that double the performance of dynamically-typed object-oriented languages. Our SELF implementation runs twice as fast as the fastest Smalltalk implementation, despite SELF's lack of classes and explicit variables.To compensate for the absence of classes, our system uses implementation-levelmaps to transparently group objects cloned from the same prototype, providing data type information and eliminating the apparent space overhead for prototype-based systems. To compensate for dynamic typing, user-defined control structures, and the lack of explicit variables, our system dynamically compilesmultiple versions of a source method, eachcustomized according to its receiver's map. Within each version the type of the receiver is fixed, and thus the compiler can statically bind andinline all messages sent toself.Message splitting andtype prediction extract and preserve even more static type information, allowing the compiler to inline many other messages. Inlining dramatically improves performance and eliminates the need to hard-wire low-level methods such as+, ==, andifTrue:.Despite inlining and other optimizations, our system still supports interactive programming environments. The system traverses internal dependency lists to invalidate all compiled methods affected by a programming change. The debugger reconstructs inlined stack frames from compiler-generated debugging information, making inlining invisible to the SELF programmer.This work has been generously supported by National Science Foundation Presidential Young Investigator Grant #CCR-8657631, and by IBM, Texas Instruments, NCR, Tandem Computers, Apple Computer, and Sun Microsystems.This paper was originally published inOOPSLA '89 Conference Proceedings (SIGPLAN Notices, 25, 10 (1989) 49–70).  相似文献   
6.
7.
8.
Chambers  B. 《Electronics letters》1994,30(19):1626-1628
The frequency characteristics of a Salisbury screen radar absorber that incorporates a layer exhibiting both resistive and capacitive properties are considered. If the capacitance is fixed, the absorber thickness and hence weight may be reduced but at the expense of bandwidth. If the capacitance is variable then the absorber may be tuned via an external control signal. The practical limitations of both applications are discussed in relation to recent advances in conducting polymer materials  相似文献   
9.
10.
The judged likelihood of a focal outcome should generally decrease as the list of alternative possibilities increases. For example, the likelihood that a runner will win a race goes down when 2 new entries are added to the field. However, 6 experiments demonstrate that the presence of implausible alternatives (duds) often increases the judged likelihood of a focal outcome. This dud-alternative effect was detected for judgments involving uncertainty about trivia facts and stochastic events. Nonnumeric likelihood measures and betting measures reliably detected the effect, but numeric likelihood measures did not. Time pressure increased the magnitude of the effect. The results were consistent with a contrast-effect account: The inclusion of duds increases the perceived strength of the evidence for the focal outcome, thereby affecting its judged likelihood. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号