首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   30篇
  免费   2篇
化学工业   1篇
轻工业   4篇
无线电   2篇
一般工业技术   1篇
冶金工业   18篇
自动化技术   6篇
  2022年   1篇
  2021年   1篇
  2016年   1篇
  2015年   1篇
  2014年   1篇
  2010年   5篇
  2008年   1篇
  2007年   1篇
  2005年   1篇
  2004年   1篇
  2003年   1篇
  2001年   1篇
  1998年   1篇
  1997年   1篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   1篇
  1993年   2篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
排序方式: 共有32条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Examined intake profiles of 54 depressed women survivors of child sexual abuse presenting for a specialized group therapy program at a community mental health center. The 32 therapy completers were compared with the 22 dropouts. Both groups possessed extensive and severe childhood and adult abuse histories. Both were extremely socially disadvantaged and clinically impaired as measured on the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory (MCMI). Dropouts were more likely to have been battered as children and to have been sexually abused only within the family. Completers were more likely to have been multiply offended both by intrafamilial and extrafamilial perpetrators. The groups differed in terms of marital status and extent of economic dependence on spouse. Dropouts appeared to be more actively enmeshed in the abuse cycle at the time of therapy. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
2.
In 2 experiments children in Grades 4–8 learned facts under different instructional conditions. Control Ss were given base sentences containing facts and were permitted to study them as they wished. In some conditions fact sentences were accompanied by elaborations (e.g., an explanation of why a stated animal lived in a stated locale). Ss in the elaborative interrogation conditions attempted to construct their own reasons for why the facts held true. Ss in the imagery conditions constructed internal imaginal representations for each fact (e.g., an image of a stated animal living in the stated locale). Elaborative interrogation was the only condition that significantly facilitated acquisition of facts in both experiments. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
3.
A long-standing belief is that statistical rules helpful in solving practical problems do not transfer beyond the subject matter domain in which they were learned. Recent research by G. T. Fong, D. H. Krantz, and R. E. Nisbett (see record 1986-29079-001) challenges this belief. Fong et al. showed that instructing learners about abstract rules, such as the law of large numbers, improved reasoning about ill-defined problems and transferred to solving everyday statistical problems that involved probabilistic relations. Fong et al.'s research is extended in 3 experiments with 276 university, secondary, and middle school students. The law-of-large-numbers heuristic was taught in regular classroom settings and students' abilities to solve ill-structured everyday problems were tested. Students learned a good deal about how to reason statistically, and these gains generalized over different structures of problems and topics. The results support a revival of formalist views of transfer, that teaching formal rules about inference making can improve reasoning and support transfer. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
4.
Although much has been discovered about relations between self-efficacy and academic achievement, questions remain about links between achievement, the structure of learning tasks, and changes in students' self-efficacy as students engage with a single, complex authentic task. Students' self-efficacy for learning (SEL) and for performance (SEP) was tracked as they worked on well- and ill-structured tasks during their regular class. Students reported higher SEL and SEP for a well-structured task. Moderate achievers reported significantly more difficulty with the ill-structured task. SEP was higher and more stable than SEL, especially in early phases of both tasks. After accounting for overall academic achievement, self-efficacy was a negligible predictor of achievement. Students may perceive various features of each task's structure as difficult. Implications concerning relations among self-efficacy, task structure, and achievement are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
5.
The authors' (1980) reanalysis of Y. S. Yarworth and W. J. Gauthier's (see record 1979-22269-001) data relating students' participation to status and self-concept variables was viewed by Gauthier and Yarworth (1980) as misconceptualized and methodologically errorful. Walsh and Winne argue here that the different approaches to regression analysis taken in Yarworth and Gauthier's original analysis of the data and in their (Walsh and Winne's) (1980) reanalysis of it should not be judged right or wrong but that the theory underlying the choice of analysis is the object of scrutiny. (4 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
6.
Dry reforming of methane was studied over Ni catalysts supported on γAl2O3, CeO2, ZrO2 and MgAl2O4 (670 °C, 1.5 bar, 16–20 l CH4 mlcatalyst−1 h−1). It is shown that MgAl2O4 supported Ni catalysts promoted with both CeO2 and ZrO2 are promising catalysts for dry reforming of methane with carbon dioxide. Within a certain composition range, the simultaneous promotion with CeO2 and ZrO2 has great influence on the amount of coke and the catalyst service time. XRD analyses indicate that formation of crystalline CexZr1−xO2 mixed oxide phases occurs on double promotion. In particular, incorporation of low amounts of Zr in the CeO2 fluorite structure provides stable dry reforming catalysis. As shown with TPR, promotion leads to a higher reduced state of Ni. SEM, XRD and TPR analyses demonstrate that highly dispersed, doubly promoted Ni catalysts with a strong metal-support interaction are essential for stable dry reforming and suppression of the formation of carbon filaments.  相似文献   
7.
We have previously described human (HsSWAP) and mouse (MmSWAP) homologs to the Drosophila alternative splicing regulator suppressor-of-white-apricot (su(wa) or DmSWAP). DmSWAP was formally defined as an alternative splicing regulator by studies showing that it autoregulates splicing of its own pre-mRNA. We report here that mammalian SWAP regulates its own splicing, and also the splicing of fibronectin and CD45. Using an in vivo system of cell transfection, mammalian SWAP regulated 5' splice site selection in splicing of its own second intron. SWAP enhanced splicing to the distal 5' splice site, whereas the SR protein ASF/SF2 enhanced splicing to the proximal site. SWAP also regulated alternative splicing of the fibronectin IIICS region by promoting exclusion of the entire IIICS region. In contrast, ASF/SF2 stimulated inclusion of the entire IIICS region. Finally, SWAP regulated splicing of CD45 exon 4, promoting exclusion of this exon, an effect also seen with ASF/SF2. Experiments using SWAP deletion mutants showed that splicing regulation of the fibronectin IIICS region and CD45 exon 4 requires a region including a carboxyl-terminal arginine/serine (R/S)-rich motif. Since R/S motifs of various splicing proteins have been shown to interact with each other, these results suggest that the R/S motif in SWAP may regulate splicing, at least in part, through interactions with other R/S containing splicing factors.  相似文献   
8.
A sensitive high-performance liquid chromatographic method is described for the quantification of midazolam and 1'-hydroxymidazolam in human plasma. Sample (1 ml plasma) preparation involved a simple solvent extraction step with a recovery of approximately 90% for both compounds. An aliquot of the dissolved residue was injected onto a 3 microm capillary C18 column (150 mm x 0.8 mm I.D.). A gradient elution was used. The initial mobile phase composition (phosphate buffer-acetonitrile, 65:35) was maintained during 16 min and was then changed linearly during a 1-min period to phosphate buffer-acetonitrile, 40:60. The flow-rate of the mobile phase was 16 microl/min and the eluate was monitored by UV detection. The limits of quantification for midazolam and 1'-hydroxymidazolam were 1 ng/ml and 0.5 ng/ml, respectively. The applicability of the method was demonstrated by studying the pharmacokinetics of midazolam, and its major metabolite 1'-hydroxymidazolam, in human volunteers following i.v. bolus administration of a subtherapeutic midazolam dose (40 microg/kg).  相似文献   
9.
The objective of this study was to evaluate the differences in biochemical, sensorial and quality characteristics of retail beef in Belgium. Four types of beef (Belgian Blue double-muscled, Limousin, Irish and Argentine) and two different muscles (longissimus lumborum and semimembranosus) were bought at the retail level and compared with regard to colour, shear force, collagen content, fatty acid analysis, taste panel evaluation as well as flavour analysis. Belgian Blue and Limousin beef had a paler colour, lower collagen and intramuscular fat contents. Fatty acid profiles were significantly different between the four types, with significantly higher PUFA/SFA and n-6/n-3 ratios for Belgiam Blue and Limousin beef compared to Argentine and Irish beef. There were significant differences between the meat types for taste panel tenderness and shear force, however both measurements did not fully correspond. Flavour analysis by gas chromatography–mass spectrometry as well as sensory analysis demonstrated that Irish and Argentine beef had a higher flavour intensity related to higher contents of volatile compounds. Differences in tenderness and flavour between the meat types were probably affected by differences in ageing time, related to import vs local production of meat.  相似文献   
10.
72 undergraduates experienced 1 of 4 repetition treatments for 1 of 3 amounts of information presented in categorized word lists. Implicitly repeating categories by presenting words not present in the original categorized list but logically members of previously studied categories significantly increased acquisition and retention relative to repeating category labels, repeating members of categories, and repeating neither labels nor members. Data on the efficiency of repetition treatments are also presented. An association-restructuring hypothesis is proposed as a cognitive mechanism enhancing learning under repetition. A hypothesis relating structural features of presented information to the availability of cognitive processes for learning under various repetition conditions is also discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号