首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1908篇
  免费   97篇
  国内免费   36篇
电工技术   14篇
综合类   133篇
化学工业   41篇
金属工艺   4篇
机械仪表   19篇
建筑科学   71篇
矿业工程   29篇
能源动力   9篇
轻工业   71篇
水利工程   5篇
石油天然气   4篇
武器工业   2篇
无线电   43篇
一般工业技术   355篇
冶金工业   921篇
自动化技术   320篇
  2025年   26篇
  2024年   51篇
  2023年   48篇
  2022年   62篇
  2021年   77篇
  2020年   59篇
  2019年   65篇
  2018年   40篇
  2017年   57篇
  2016年   39篇
  2015年   34篇
  2014年   54篇
  2013年   71篇
  2012年   54篇
  2011年   141篇
  2010年   122篇
  2009年   138篇
  2008年   113篇
  2007年   110篇
  2006年   103篇
  2005年   108篇
  2004年   109篇
  2003年   98篇
  2002年   81篇
  2001年   63篇
  2000年   14篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   7篇
  1991年   3篇
  1989年   4篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   2篇
  1966年   5篇
  1965年   6篇
  1964年   8篇
  1963年   5篇
  1961年   4篇
  1960年   5篇
  1959年   4篇
  1958年   4篇
  1957年   2篇
  1956年   5篇
  1955年   4篇
  1954年   2篇
排序方式: 共有2041条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
In long-term memory, negative information is better remembered than neutral information. Differences in processes important to working memory may contribute to this emotional memory enhancement. To examine the effect that the emotional content of stimuli has on working memory performance, the authors asked participants to perform working memory tasks with negative and neutral stimuli. Task accuracy was unaffected by the emotional content of the stimuli. Reaction times also did not differ for negative relative to neutral words, but on an n-back task using faces, participants were slower to respond to fearful faces than to neutral faces. These results suggest that although emotional content does not have a robust effect on working memory, in some instances emotional salience can impede working memory performance. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
2.
Replies to comments made by G. E. Gignac (see record 2005-06671-010) on the current authors' original article (see record 2003-02341-015). Gignac reanalyzed the factor structure of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) and found results that differed from those the authors obtained initially. The authors tracked down the surprising sources of those discrepancies. G. E. Gignac's hierarchical model of emotional intelligence appears promising, and the authors anticipate that further investigations of the MSCEIT factor structure may yield additional information. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
3.
Researchers often have expectations about the research outcomes in regard to inequality constraints between, e.g., group means. Consider the example of researchers who investigated the effects of inducing a negative emotional state in aggressive boys. It was expected that highly aggressive boys would, on average, score higher on aggressive responses toward other peers than moderately aggressive boys, who would in turn score higher than nonaggressive boys. In most cases, null hypothesis testing is used to evaluate such hypotheses. We show, however, that hypotheses formulated using inequality constraints between the group means are generally not evaluated properly. The wrong hypotheses are tested, i.e.. the null hypothesis that group means are equal. In this article, we propose an innovative solution to these above-mentioned issues using Bayesian model selection, which we illustrate using a case study. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
4.
Reports an error in Age differences in psychosocial predictors of positive and negative affect: A longitudinal investigation of young, midlife, and older adults by Tim D. Windsor and Kaarin J. Anstey (Psychology and Aging, 2010[Sep], Vol 25[3], 641-652). Contains an error in Figure 3, on page 649. The correction discusses where to find the correct data. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2010-18944-009.) Research has consistently shown that despite aging-related losses, older adults have high levels of emotional well-being relative to those in young and midlife adults. We aimed to contribute to knowledge around the factors that predict emotional well-being over the life course by examining age group differences in associations of positive and negative social exchanges and mastery beliefs with positive and negative affect in a sample of 7,472 young, midlife, and older adults assessed on 2 measurement occasions, 4 years apart. Results from structural equation models indicated lower levels of negative affect with advancing age. Mastery was consistently related to higher well-being, with the strongest associations evident for young adults. Older adults reported the most frequent positive and least frequent negative social exchanges; however, associations of social relations with affect tended to be stronger among young and midlife adults relative to older adults. Results are discussed in the context of life course perspectives on goal orientations and self-regulatory processes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
5.
In the present research, we test the assumption that emotional mimicry and contagion are moderated by group membership. We report two studies using facial electromyography (EMG; Study 1), Facial Action Coding System (FACS; Study 2), and self-reported emotions (Study 2) as dependent measures. As predicted, both studies show that ingroup anger and fear displays were mimicked to a greater extent than outgroup displays of these emotions. The self-report data in Study 2 further showed specific divergent reactions to outgroup anger and fear displays. Outgroup anger evoked fear, and outgroup fear evoked aversion. Interestingly, mimicry increased liking for ingroup models but not for outgroup models. The findings are discussed in terms of the social functions of emotions in group contexts. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
6.
基于HMM的可训练情感语音合成研究   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
陈洁  张雪英  孙颖 《电声技术》2012,36(3):43-46
研究了基于HMM的英语语音合成系统,并通过分析情感语句的语调,得到高兴、生气、悲伤、平静四种不同情感类型的基音频率、能量和时长的变化规律。将基于HMM的可训练语音合成方法应用到英语情感语音合成,对基于HMM所合成的平静语音,叠加相应情感的语调变化规则,通过修改韵律特征参数合成出带有情感的语音。从所合成情感语句的效果来看,合成语音整体稳定流畅、情感特征明显,且合成系统可以快速构成。  相似文献   
7.
为了解18~45岁女性消费者在购买运动文胸时的情感偏好,以消费者在穿着体验中的评价为依据进行问卷调研,衡量消费者的情感偏好。通过探索性因子分析和验证性因子分析,构建5个一级指标和21个二级指标的消费者情感偏好评价指标模型,采用非线性主成分分析法确定影响消费者情感偏好设计要素的权重值。结果表明:一级指标权重值从大到小依次为面料(0.274)、色彩(0.191)、背部结构(0.188)、肩带形式(0.184)、罩杯形式(0.163),说明面料是消费者购买运动文胸时考虑的主要因素。  相似文献   
8.
探讨一些地下车库排烟设计方法。并着重说明了设计数据方面的两项重要变化。同时探讨了地下车库排烟系统与排风系统完全合一的可能性。  相似文献   
9.
李国英  罗怡  张铭让 《中国皮革》2000,29(23):23-26
本文对采用 Michael加成反应合成出的 LL - I醛酸助鞣剂改性皮胶原 ,以提高铬与胶原的结合量的性能特点及其应用工艺进行了讨论。  相似文献   
10.
    
ABSTRACT

In recent years, a growing interest has been created for improvement of human interaction with computers. Hence, automatic recognition of facial expressions has become one of the active research topics. The purpose of this paper is to identify facial expressions, by using differential geometric features. In the proposed method, only the first and last images are used and differential features are extracted from these two images. Differential geometric features are extracted from changes in the important points of the face in the two images. In this method, the distance between the important points of the face and the reference point was calculated in both directions x and y, for two images, and with the difference between the distance, the differential geometric features between the two images were obtained. Based on the results, with this method, recognition accuracy of six facial expressions in the database was 96.44%, CK +.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号