首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   283篇
  免费   36篇
  国内免费   28篇
电工技术   5篇
综合类   38篇
化学工业   1篇
机械仪表   7篇
建筑科学   20篇
矿业工程   5篇
能源动力   4篇
轻工业   8篇
水利工程   24篇
石油天然气   27篇
武器工业   2篇
无线电   4篇
一般工业技术   8篇
冶金工业   112篇
原子能技术   3篇
自动化技术   79篇
  2024年   2篇
  2023年   9篇
  2022年   14篇
  2021年   8篇
  2020年   8篇
  2019年   8篇
  2018年   8篇
  2017年   1篇
  2016年   5篇
  2015年   11篇
  2014年   13篇
  2013年   9篇
  2012年   12篇
  2011年   28篇
  2010年   22篇
  2009年   30篇
  2008年   17篇
  2007年   23篇
  2006年   22篇
  2005年   19篇
  2004年   7篇
  2003年   16篇
  2002年   9篇
  2001年   9篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   1篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   2篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   1篇
  1989年   3篇
  1986年   1篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   1篇
  1978年   2篇
  1977年   1篇
  1974年   2篇
  1971年   1篇
  1964年   1篇
  1963年   1篇
  1960年   2篇
  1959年   1篇
  1955年   1篇
排序方式: 共有347条查询结果,搜索用时 62 毫秒
81.
The authors propose that correction of dispositional inferences involves the examination of situational constraints and the suppression of dispositional inferences. They hypothesized that suppression would result in dispositional rebound. In Study 1, participants saw a video of either a free or a forced speaker. Participants shown a forced speaker later made stronger dispositional inferences about a 2nd, free speaker than control participants did. Study 2 provided evidence for higher rebound among participants who reported trying harder to suppress dispositional inferences during the 1st video. In Study 3, participants were asked to focus on situational constraints or to avoid thinking about the speaker's characteristics. Only the latter instructions led to a dispositional rebound. These data support the view that the correction of dispositional inferences involves 2 processes that lead to distinct consequences in subsequent attribution work. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
82.
提出了制造系统的八面体总体决策框架模型.以成本、时间、质量、柔性、环境性和安全性作为制造系统总体优化的决策属性目标,通过恰当地选取决策变量和建立技术经济模型,完成从各个决策变量到六大决策属性的映射,使整个决策过程建立在科学根据和科学分析的基础上,从而实现各个制造决策属性的总体优化.  相似文献   
83.
Foodborne pathogens are responsible for an increasing burden of disease worldwide. Knowledge on the contribution of different food sources and water for disease is essential to prioritize food safety interventions and implement appropriate control measures. Source attribution using outbreak data utilizes readily available data from outbreak surveillance to estimate the contribution of different sources to human disease. We developed a probabilistic model based on outbreak data that attributes human foodborne disease by various bacterial pathogens to sources in Latin America and the Caribbean (LA&C). Foods implicated in outbreaks were classified by their ingredients as simple foods (i.e. belonging to one single food category), or complex foods (i.e. belonging to multiple food categories). For each agent, the data from simple-food outbreaks were summarized, and the proportion of outbreaks caused by each category was used to define the probability that an outbreak was caused by a source. For the calculation of the number of outbreaks attributed to each source, simple-food outbreaks were attributed to the single food category in question, and complex-food outbreaks were partitioned to each category proportionally to the estimated probability. We analysed all bacterial pathogens together, focused on important bacterial pathogens separately, and, when data were sufficient, performed analyses by country, decade and location. Between 1993 and 2010, 6313 bacterial outbreaks were reported by 20 countries. In general, the most important sources of bacterial disease were meat, dairy products, water and vegetables in the 1990s, and eggs, vegetables, and grains and beans in the 2000s. We observed fluctuations of the most important sources of disease for each pathogen between decades and countries, which may be a consequence of changes in the control of zoonotic disease over the years, of changes in food consumption habits, or of changes in public health focus and availability of data of different pathogens. This study identified data gaps in the region and highlighted the importance of effective surveillance systems to identify sources of disease. Still, the application of this method for source attribution in the LA&C region was successful, and we concluded that this approach can be used to attribute disease to food sources and water in other regions, including developing regions with limited data on the public health impact of foodborne diseases.  相似文献   
84.
陈亮  杨庚  屠袁飞 《计算机应用》2016,36(7):1822-1827
针对现有云存储的数据和访问控制的安全性不高,从而造成用户存储的敏感信息被盗取的现象,结合现有的基于密文策略属性加密(CP-ABE)方案和数据分割的思想,提出了一个基于混合云的高效数据隐私保护模型。首先根据用户数据的敏感程度将数据合理分割成不同敏感级别的数据块,将分割后的数据存储在不同的云平台上,再根据数据的安全级别,进行不同强度的加密技术进行数据加密。同时在敏感信息解密阶段采取“先匹配后解密”的方法,并对算法进行了优化,最后用户进行一个乘法运算解密得到明文。在公有云中对1 Gb数据进行对称加密,较单节点提高了效率一倍多。实验结果表明:该方案可以有效保护云存储用户的隐私数据,同时降低了系统的开销,提高了灵活性。  相似文献   
85.
86.
The authors explored the relationship between managerial influence tactics and employee resistance to organizational change. Using attribution theory, the authors developed a series of hypotheses concerning the effects of influence tactics on employee resistance to change and the ways in which these relationships are moderated by leader-member exchange. Results, which are based on multisource data, suggest that employee resistance reflects both the type of influence a manager uses and the strength of leader-member exchange. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
87.
Many studies have demonstrated that infants can attribute goals to observed actions, whether they are presented live by familiar agents or on a computer screen by abstract figures. However, because most, if not all, of these studies rely on the repeated action presentations typical of infant studies, it is not clear whether infants are simply recognizing the completed action as goal directed, or whether they can productively infer a not-yet-achieved outcome from an ongoing action. We investigated this question by presenting 13-month-old infants with a single animated chasing event. Infants looked longer at the outcome of this action when, given the opportunity, the chaser did not catch the chasee than when it did. Crucially, this result was dependent on whether the action could be construed as efficient with regard to this goal state. This finding suggests the ability to infer the goal of an ongoing novel action and illustrates the productivity of 1-year-olds’ action understanding. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
88.
Early studies of human memory suggest that adherence to a known structural regularity (e.g., orthographic regularity) benefits memory for an otherwise novel stimulus (e.g., G. A. Miller, 1958). However, a more recent study suggests that structural regularity can lead to an increase in false-positive responses on recognition memory tests (B. W. A. Whittlesea & L. D. Williams, 1998). In the present study the authors attempted to identify the circumstances under which structural regularity benefits old-new discrimination and those under which it leads to an increase in false-positive responses. The highly generalizable tendency shown here is for structural regularity to benefit old-new discrimination. The increase in false-positive responses for structurally regular novel items may be limited to situations in which regularity is confounded with similarity to studied items. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
89.
Group formation is an inevitable consequence of social life, and the tendency to perceive people as a collective unit persists once they have been categorized as a group. Drawing on the concept of homogeneity, the authors propose a model suggesting that groups may endure in part because people who are perceived as homogeneous attract collective treatment (e.g., monetary rewards and punishment), and such treatment further reinforces the perception that the group’s members are homogeneous. In support of this model, more homogeneous groups attracted collective treatment and collectively treated groups seemed to be more homogeneous thereafter. The authors suggest that these effects arise in part because people intuitively believe that group homogeneity is associated with collective treatment, and they present evidence suggesting that this applies to at least one policy-relevant real-world setting. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
90.
The present study compared 20 patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer's disease with 20 older controls (ages 69-94 years) on their ability to make inferences about emotions and beliefs in others. Six tasks tested their ability to make 1st-order and 2nd-order inferences as well as to offer explanations and moral evaluations of human action by appeal to emotions and beliefs. Results showed that the ability to infer emotions and beliefs in 1st-order tasks remains largely intact in patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer's. Patients were able to use mental states in the prediction, explanation, and moral evaluation of behavior. Impairment on 2nd-order tasks involving inference of mental states was equivalent to impairment on control tasks, suggesting that patients' difficulty is secondary to their cognitive impairments. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号